Publication Date
In 2025 | 41 |
Since 2024 | 738 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 2909 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 6529 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 14910 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 969 |
Teachers | 865 |
Researchers | 621 |
Parents | 492 |
Policymakers | 204 |
Administrators | 124 |
Students | 82 |
Community | 57 |
Media Staff | 31 |
Counselors | 20 |
Support Staff | 17 |
More ▼ |
Location
Australia | 894 |
Canada | 481 |
United Kingdom (England) | 415 |
United Kingdom | 405 |
United States | 360 |
New Zealand | 277 |
California | 274 |
China | 273 |
Turkey | 242 |
Netherlands | 204 |
Sweden | 178 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 20 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 26 |
Does not meet standards | 28 |

Rholes, William S.; Ruble, Diane N. – Child Development, 1984
In the studies, subjects from different age groups (5-6, 7-8, 9-10, 18-22 years) observed vignettes designed to reveal an actor's abilities or personality traits, and were told about one instance of an actor's behavior, respectively. The subjects were then asked to predict the actor's behavior in related situations. Results focused differences in…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior, Children, Comparative Analysis

Graham, Sandra; And Others – Child Development, 1984
Children between the ages of six and 11 were asked to recall personal experiences of pity, anger, and guilt and to rate the cause of each emotion on degree of controllability. Results were interpreted as evidence that guilt in young children may be a qualitatively different emotion because of its closer link to outcome than to perceived…
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Age Differences, Children, Cognitive Development

Gathercole, Virginia C. – Journal of Child Language, 1985
Describes a study designed to discover how children approach the mass-count distinction as it applies to the use of "much" and "many." Results indicate that children do not approach the co-occurrence conditions of "much" and "many" with various nouns from a semantic point of view, but rather from a…
Descriptors: Adjectives, Child Language, Language Acquisition, Morphemes

Stoel-Gammon, Carol – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1985
Longitudinal samples of meaningful speech of 34 normally developing children were analyzed to determine the range and types of consonantal phones produced at 15, 18, 21, and 24 months. Findings are related to other longitudinal research in early phonological development and to studies of babbling and correct productions. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Child Development, Infants, Language Acquisition, Longitudinal Studies
Hind, Jim – Gifted Education International, 1984
The article argues that even extremely young children can be taught to program microcomputers from their very first contact. A teaching strategy is proposed, having more in common with the teaching of language than with the more traditional didactic-reinforcement cycle commonly employed in the text books. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Computer Literacy, Computer Software, Language Acquisition, Microcomputers

Smith, Linda B. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1985
Examined whether a holistic magnitude relation governs children's object comparisons. Objects varying on two dimensions of magnitude, size, and saturation were classified by three-, four-, and five-year-olds. Results indicated that younger children were sensitive to global magnitude as well as to overall similarity. (Author/BE)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Classification, Developmental Stages, Holistic Approach

Guttentag, Robert E. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1985
Three experiments tested for developmental changes in attention to auditory and visual signals. Results showed that adults and seven-year-olds tended to allocate their attention to vision rather than audition when no precue was provided. While not entirely consistent, results with four-year-olds suggested a similar biasing of attention to vision.…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Attention, Auditory Stimuli

Allen, K. Eileen – Topics in Early Childhood Special Education, 1984
Federal legislation related to young handicapped children enacted during the past 20 years is reviewed in terms of the history and current status of several of the most effective and enduring of the early childhood programs. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Disabilities, Early Childhood Education, Federal Legislation, History

Bloom, Lois; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1984
The acquisition of "to" in infinitive complement structure was examined in the spontaneous speech data from four children who were observed longitudinally from two to three years of age. Results support the conclusion that the verb system is a determining factor in the acquisition of linguistic structure. (SED)
Descriptors: Child Language, Language Acquisition, Language Research, Semantics

Axia, Giovanna; Baroni, Maria Rosa – Child Development, 1985
Capacity to react to the cost of a request in relation to interlocutor's behavior is acquired early; ability to maintain good interactions by increasing the politeness of a request occurs later. Only from 9 years on do children use linguistic politeness as a criterion in judging a request's appropriateness according to addressee's status. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Communication Problems, Communicative Competence (Languages), Interpersonal Competence

Smith, Robin; And Others – Child Development, 1985
Despite assertions to the contrary, preschool children are capable of understanding cinematic events conveyed through camera techniques and film editing. This ability nevertheless substantially increases with age among children from four- to seven-years-old. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Ability, Comprehension, Films

Tunmer, William E. – Child Development, 1985
Acquisition of sentient-nonsentient distinction in 48 children between four- and seven-years-of-age occurred later than animate-inanimate distinction. The children's use of naturalistic or nonnaturalistic explanations depended on the logical nature of events in which objects were involved rather than familiarity with objects themselves. Ability to…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Comprehension, Concept Formation, Foreign Countries

Anderson, Daniel R.; And Others – Child Development, 1985
Describes a new observational study of home television viewing by young children which involved placement of time-lapse video cameras in the homes of five-year-olds from middle-class families for a 10-day period. Families maintained TV viewing diaries, and control groups of families were employed to assess the impact of observational equipment in…
Descriptors: Diaries, Estimation (Mathematics), Parents, Questionnaires

Swift, Carolyn F.; And Others – Mental Retardation, 1985
Two groups of professionals, one serving specific regions and the other administering statewide programs, agreed that public sources of funds are the most available for early intervention programs, that the most effective services are performed as early as possible and that rural and poor populations are underserved. (Author/CL)
Descriptors: Disabilities, Financial Support, Infants, Intervention
Dyson, Lily; Fewell, Rebecca R. – Journal of the Division for Early Childhood, 1986
Findings of the study involving 15 families with handicapped children (three to six years old) and 15 without indicate significantly greater stress in families with handicapped children. Four factors are identified as significantly related to stress: (1) child characteristics, (2) physical incapacity, (3) parental pessimism regarding the child,…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Disabilities, Elementary Education, Family Relationship