Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 15 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 80 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 272 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1017 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 149 |
Teachers | 87 |
Researchers | 31 |
Students | 21 |
Administrators | 19 |
Support Staff | 6 |
Policymakers | 3 |
Community | 2 |
Counselors | 2 |
Parents | 1 |
Location
Florida | 14 |
California | 10 |
Ohio | 10 |
Missouri | 9 |
Vermont | 9 |
Germany | 8 |
Canada | 7 |
Illinois | 7 |
Australia | 6 |
Georgia | 6 |
Spain | 6 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Elementary and Secondary… | 7 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 2 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Ingraham v Wright | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Haughey, Michael D. – ASHRAE Journal, 2003
Describes installation of a hybrid chilled water/ice thermal storage plant for the Lucile Erwin Middle School in Loveland, Colorado, which has saved more than $18,000 in energy costs annually. (EV)
Descriptors: Air Conditioning Equipment, Climate Control, Energy Conservation, School Buildings

Khattar, Mukesh; Shirey, Don, III; Raustad, Richard – ASHRAE Journal, 2003
Describes how the Brevard County School District in Florida teamed with companies EPRI and Florida Power and Light to implement a dual-path, low temperature air-distribution system used in conjunction with thermal energy storage. (EV)
Descriptors: Air Conditioning Equipment, Climate Control, Energy Conservation, School Buildings

Kymissis, Effie; Poulson, Claire L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1994
Five male preschool children were studied to determine the extent to which the generalized motor imitation model applies to vocal imitation. The use of a reinforced-imitation condition produced a systematic increase in verbal matching responses among three of the five children, which is consistent with previous studies. (SW)
Descriptors: Conditioning, Imitation, Observational Learning, Preschool Children

Saunders, Richard R.; Chaney, Lisa; Marquis, Janet G. – Psychological Record, 2005
In Experiment 1, 12 senior citizens from the community were trained with 18 sets of conditional discriminations. Training included 2-, 3-, and 4-choice matching-to-sample (MTS) configurations in linear series (LS), many-to-one (MTO), and one-to-many (OTM) training structures. Training structure order was counterbalanced across participants. The…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Probability, Older Adults, Training
Amundson, J.C.; Wheeler, D.S.; Miller, R.R. – Learning and Motivation, 2005
In two conditioned lick suppression experiments using water-deprived rats, we examined the effects of following Pavlovian conditioned inhibition training (i.e., A-US/AX-NoUS) with pairings of the training excitor (A) and the unconditioned stimulus (US). Experiments 1 and 2 assessed the effects of this posttraining inflation treatment on Pavlovian…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Economic Climate, Inhibition, Classical Conditioning
Thomas, Brian L.; Ayres, John J. B. – Learning and Motivation, 2004
In four experiments using albino rats in an ABA fear renewal paradigm, we studied conditioned fear in the A test context following extinction in Context B. Conditioned suppression of operant responding was the index of fear. In Experiments 1-3, we found that extinguishing a feared cue in compound with a putative conditioned inhibitor of fear led…
Descriptors: Fear, Learning Processes, Animals, Cues
Ward-Robinson, Jasper – Learning and Motivation, 2004
Three mechanisms can explain second-order conditioning: (1) The second-order conditioned stimulus (CS2) could activate a representation of the first-order conditioned stimulus (CS1), thereby provoking the conditioned response (CR); The CS2 could enter into an excitatory association with either (2) the representation governing the CR, or (3) with a…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Stimuli, Reinforcement, Animals
Gibson, Evelyn; Reed, Phil – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2005
The present study explored whether a similar phenomenon to stimulus over-selectivity occurred in rats, in the hope of establishing a non-human model for the autism. Rats were serially presented with two-15 seconds, two-element compound stimuli prior to the delivery of food, in an appetitive classical conditioning procedure. Each compound stimulus…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Animals, Responses, Conditioning
Palmer, David C. – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2005
In this article, the author shares an anecdote demonstrating one-trial learning which is commonplace in human behavior. The demonstration suggests that under some conditions, when people hear someone speak, their behavior changes, even in the absence of an apparent contingency of reinforcement, but only if they have in their repertoire verbal…
Descriptors: Behavior Change, Reinforcement, Listening, Learning
Simon, Barbara B.; Knuckley, Bryan; Powell, Donald A. – Learning & Memory, 2004
Previous work has demonstrated that drugs increasing brain concentrations of acetylcholine can enhance cognition in aging and brain-damaged organisms. The present study assessed whether galantamine (GAL), an allosteric modulator of nicotinic cholinergic receptors and weak acetylcholinesterase inhibitor, could improve acquisition and retention of…
Descriptors: Classical Conditioning, Animals, Eye Movements, Biochemistry
Blouin, Ashley M.; Cain, Chris K.; Barad, Mike – Learning & Memory, 2004
Having recently shown that extinction of conditioned fear depends on L-type voltage-gated calcium channels (LVGCCs), we have been seeking other protocols that require this unusual induction mechanism. We tested latent inhibition (LI) of fear, because LI resembles extinction except that cue exposures precede, rather than follow, cue-shock pairing.…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Animals, Cytology, Neurology
Lebron, Kelimer; Milad, Mohammed R.; Quirk, Gregory J. – Learning & Memory, 2004
Extinction of auditory fear conditioning is thought to form a new memory. We previously found that rats with vmPFC lesions could extinguish fear to the tone within a session, but showed no recall of extinction 24 h later. One interpretation is that the vmPFC is the sole storage site of extinction memory. However, it is also possible that lesioned…
Descriptors: Memory, Fear, Animals, Brain
Gould, Thomas J.; Lewis, Michael C. – Learning & Memory, 2005
The present study investigated the hypothesis that both nicotinic acetylcholinergic receptors (nAChRs) and glutamate receptors ([alpha]-amino-3-hydroxy-5-methyl-4-isoxazole propionate receptors (AMPARs) and N-methyl-D-aspartate glutamate receptors (NMDARs)) are involved in fear conditioning, and may modulate similar processes. The effects of the…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Inhibition, Fear, Context Effect
de Graaff, Saskia; Verhoeven, Ludo; Bosman, Anna M. T.; Hasselman, Fred – British Journal of Educational Psychology, 2007
Background: The conclusion from a vast literature on literacy acquisition is that letter knowledge is one of the best predictors of literacy development. The question of the best way to teach children letter sounds has not, as yet, been answered satisfactorily. Aims: The aim of this study was the evaluation of a computer training program using…
Descriptors: Mnemonics, Early Childhood Education, Kindergarten, Preschool Evaluation
Tonneau, Francois; Arreola, Fara; Martinez, Alma Gabriela – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
In studies of function transformation, participants initially are taught to match stimuli in the presence of a contextual cue, X; the stimuli to be matched bear some formal relation to each other, for example, a relation of opposition or difference. In a second phase, the participants are taught to match arbitrary stimuli (say, A and B) in the…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Cues, Objective Tests, Classical Conditioning