Publication Date
In 2025 | 2 |
Since 2024 | 63 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 194 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 451 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 862 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 188 |
Teachers | 123 |
Students | 44 |
Researchers | 38 |
Parents | 2 |
Administrators | 1 |
Community | 1 |
Media Staff | 1 |
Location
China | 33 |
Australia | 28 |
Canada | 24 |
Indonesia | 24 |
Japan | 24 |
Netherlands | 18 |
South Korea | 17 |
Germany | 15 |
Taiwan | 15 |
United Kingdom (England) | 15 |
Turkey | 14 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
National Defense Education… | 4 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 2 |
Bilingual Education Act 1968 | 1 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Head Start | 1 |
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 2 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 2 |
Bloom, Lois – Contemp Psychol, 1970
A book describing children's utterances in terms of similarities to and deviations from the "well-formed structures of the adult model is reviewed. (DB)
Descriptors: Child Language, Linguistic Theory, Sentence Structure

Drake, Dana B.; And Others – Hispania, 1982
Discusses when an infinitive may appear directly after a noun in Spanish and which word is used and why if a preposition or other such word is required. Examples used are where the infinitive phrase is the predicate nominative with the verb "ser," where the infinitive acts as the grammatical subject, and where the infinitive phrase is a…
Descriptors: Grammar, Nouns, Semantics, Sentence Structure

Dowling, Lee H. – Hispania, 1981
Presents study which shows that although these causatives have same surface structure as productive causative constructions they differ in several ways, e.g., unlike other productive causatives their meaning changes when "que" subjunctive is substituted for the infinitive, and they function like lexical causatives which involve agent-patient, not…
Descriptors: Grammar, Romance Languages, Sentence Structure, Spanish

Boguslavskaya, Galina P. – English Language Teaching Journal, 1981
Distinguishes among three different adverbial modifiers--those of purpose, of antipurpose, and of subsequent events. Suggests this delineation leads to a better understanding of English texts. (Author/BK)
Descriptors: Adverbs, English (Second Language), Sentence Structure

Cruse, D. A. – Journal of Linguistics, 1979
Examines the question of the transitivity of the part-whole relation in grammar, in the context of the sentence and its semantic structure. (AM)
Descriptors: Grammar, Semantics, Sentence Structure, Structural Analysis

Dai, John Xiang-Ling. – Journal of Chinese Linguistics, 1992
Analysis of six cross-linguistic properties characterizing the head verb in the resultative construction in Chinese shows that the first verb, and not the second verb, should be analyzed as the head verb. (15 references) (Author/CB)
Descriptors: Chinese, Semantics, Sentence Structure, Syntax

Khoo, Christopher S. G.; Dai, Yubin; Loh, Teck Ee – Journal of the American Society for Information Science and Technology, 2002
Describes the development of new statistical formulas for identifying two- and three-character words in Chinese text by performing stepwise logistic regression using a sample of sentences that had been manually segmented. Concludes that the new contextual information formulas are substantially better than the mutual information formula.…
Descriptors: Chinese, Mathematical Formulas, Sentence Structure, Statistics

Kittila, Seppo – Language Sciences, 2002
Discusses issues important to the study of the typology of transitivity. Data from numerous languages are presented to show what parameters can contribute to the linguistic expression of transitive events typologically. (Author/VWL)
Descriptors: Grammar, Language Typology, Languages, Sentence Structure
Li, Wendan – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2004
The topic chain is a frequently used structure in Chinese. In this article, its structural characteristics and its use in written narrative text are examined. Three major claims are made: (a) The topic of a chain does not have to overtly occur in the chain-initial clause as commonly believed; (b) depending on the context, a zero noun phrase (NP)…
Descriptors: Sentence Structure, Nouns, Phrase Structure, Chinese
Kaan, Edith; Wijnen, Frank; Swaab, Tamara Y. – Brain and Language, 2004
In the present study we use event related potentials (ERPs) to explore the time course of identification and resolution of verb gaps. ERPs were recorded while participants read sentences that contained a verb gap like "Ron took/sanded the planks, and Bill O the hammer"... Plausibility of the critical words ("hammer") that followed the verb gap was…
Descriptors: Verbs, Comprehension, Language Processing, Sentence Structure
Demestre, Josep; Garcia-Albea, Jose E. – Cognitive Science, 2007
Event-related brain potentials were recorded while subjects listened to sentences containing a controlled infinitival complement. Subject and object control items were used, both with 2 potential antecedents in the upper clause. Half of the sentences had a gender agreement violation between the null subject of the infinitival complement and an…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Neurolinguistics, Language Processing, Error Analysis (Language)
Roland, Douglas; Dick, Frederic; Elman, Jeffrey L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Many recent models of language comprehension have stressed the role of distributional frequencies in determining the relative accessibility or ease of processing associated with a particular lexical item or sentence structure. However, there exist relatively few comprehensive analyses of structural frequencies, and little consideration has been…
Descriptors: Sentence Structure, Psycholinguistics, Grammar, Child Language
Liu, Yanni; Shu, Hua; Wei, Jinghan – Brain and Language, 2006
Two event-related potential (ERP) experiments were conducted to investigate spoken word recognition in Chinese and the effect of contextual constraints on this process. In Experiment 1, three kinds of incongruous words were formed by altering the first, second or both syllables of the congruous disyllabic terminal words in high constraint spoken…
Descriptors: Rhyme, Phonetics, Sentence Structure, Oral Language
Fisher, Cynthia; Klingler, Stacy L.; Song, Hyun-joo – Cognition, 2006
Children as young as two use sentence structure to learn the meanings of verbs. We probed the generality of sensitivity to sentence structure by moving to a different semantic and syntactic domain, spatial prepositions. Twenty-six-month-olds used sentence structure to determine whether a new word was an object-category name ("This is a corp!") or…
Descriptors: Sentence Structure, Form Classes (Languages), Toddlers, Language Acquisition
Noel, Kathryn; Edmunds, Alan L. – Roeper Review, 2006
Conceptual models of giftedness that cannot explain the full spectrum of depicted behaviors are incomplete. The same is true for frameworks that purport to analyze those behaviors. Within giftedness, extraordinary and rare talents afford us the opportunity to extend and modify models and frameworks. This article presents an analysis of the first 2…
Descriptors: Gifted, Models, Comparative Analysis, Children