Publication Date
In 2025 | 3 |
Since 2024 | 22 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 88 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 219 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 532 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 123 |
Parents | 55 |
Practitioners | 32 |
Teachers | 10 |
Support Staff | 4 |
Community | 1 |
Students | 1 |
Location
Japan | 29 |
Canada | 26 |
United States | 26 |
Australia | 22 |
Sweden | 21 |
Germany | 18 |
United Kingdom (England) | 17 |
France | 14 |
Netherlands | 11 |
Israel | 10 |
New York (New York) | 9 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Education of the Handicapped… | 1 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Brand, Rebecca J.; Shallcross, Wendy L.; Sabatos, Maura G.; Massie, Kara Phaedra – Infancy, 2007
Mothers modify their actions when demonstrating objects to infants versus adults. Such modifications have been called infant-directed action (IDA) or "motionese" (Brand, Baldwin, & Ashburn, 2002). We investigated the IDA features of interactiveness and simplification by quantifying eye gaze, object exchanges, and action units enacted…
Descriptors: Mothers, Eye Movements, Infants, Motion
Schmuckler, Mark A.; Collimore, Lisa M.; Dannemiller, James L. – Infancy, 2007
This experiment investigated the impact of the path of approach of an object, from head on versus from the side, and the type of imminent contact with that object, a hit versus a miss, on young infants' perceptions of object looming. Consistent with earlier studies, we found that 4- to 5-month-old infants do indeed discriminate hits versus misses.…
Descriptors: Infants, Experiments, Visual Perception, Infant Behavior
D'Entremont, Barbara; Yazbek, Aimee – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2007
To determine whether children with autism (CWA) would selectively imitate intentional, as opposed to accidental actions, an experimenter demonstrated either an "intentional" and an "accidental" action or two "intentional" actions on the same toy [Carpenter, Akhtar, & Tomasello ("1998a") "Infant Behavior and Development, 21," 315-330]. CWA tended…
Descriptors: Children, Infant Behavior, Social Cognition, Developmental Delays
Teinonen, Tuomas; Aslin, Richard N.; Alku, Paavo; Csibra, Gergely – Cognition, 2008
Previous research has shown that infants match vowel sounds to facial displays of vowel articulation [Kuhl, P. K., & Meltzoff, A. N. (1982). The bimodal perception of speech in infancy. "Science, 218", 1138-1141; Patterson, M. L., & Werker, J. F. (1999). Matching phonetic information in lips and voice is robust in 4.5-month-old infants. "Infant…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Phonetics, Vowels, Phonemics
Mills, Rosemary S. L.; Imm, Gorette P.; Walling, Bobbi R.; Weiler, Hope A. – Developmental Psychology, 2008
The purpose of this study was to characterize cortisol response and regulation associated with shame responding in early childhood and to examine how general the relation between shame and cortisol is. It was predicted that children responding to task failure with shame would show a larger and more prolonged cortisol response than other children.…
Descriptors: Infants, Infant Behavior, Children, Gender Differences
Adolph, Karen E.; Robinson, Scott R.; Young, Jesse W.; Gill-Alvarez, Felix – Psychological Review, 2008
Developmental trajectories provide the empirical foundation for theories about change processes during development. However, the ability to distinguish among alternative trajectories depends on how frequently observations are sampled. This study used real behavioral data, with real patterns of variability, to examine the effects of sampling at…
Descriptors: Intervals, Child Development, Sampling, Infant Behavior
Luciano, Carmen; Becerra, Inmaculada Gomez; Valverde, Miguel Rodriguez – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
The conditions under which symmetry and equivalence relations develop are still controversial. This paper reports three experiments that attempt to analyze the impact of multiple-exemplar training (MET) in receptive symmetry on the emergence of visual-visual equivalence relations with a very young child, Gloria. At the age of 15 months 24 days…
Descriptors: Infants, Females, Naming, Training
Parker-Rees, Rod – Early Years: An International Journal of Research and Development, 2007
This paper offers a review of the literature on the role of imitation in the earliest stages of social interaction between babies and familiar partners. The review focuses on the ways in which reciprocal imitation marks familiar relationships that provide special contexts for babies to engage actively and exuberantly in the construction of a…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Imitation, Infants, Interpersonal Relationship
Gonzalez-Mena, Janet – Young Children, 2007
Hungarian pediatrician Emmi Pikler theorized that freedom of movement facilitates infants' development and learning. The self-education promoted by freedom to move gives an infant a lasting view of herself as a competent learner. Pikler's approach also emphasizes the importance of helping each child feel respected and secure. The author examines…
Descriptors: Caregivers, Problem Solving, Hunger, Infants
Fagan, Mary K.; Iverson, Jana M. – Infancy, 2007
Although vocalization and mouthing are behaviors frequently performed by infants, little is known about the characteristics of vocalizations that occur with objects, hands, or fingers in infants' mouths. The purpose of this research was to investigate characteristics of vocalizations associated with mouthing in 6 to 9-month-old infants during play…
Descriptors: Phonetics, Infants, Infant Behavior, Language Acquisition
McMullen, Mary Benson – Early Childhood Research & Practice, 2010
This reflective essay describes the author's experiences as an observer in a behaviorist infant classroom. The author developed four categories of practice to describe what happened in the behaviorist infant room: (1) curricular focus on training typically developing infants to meet typical developmental milestones, (2) the use of highly…
Descriptors: Infants, Constructivism (Learning), Observation, Child Care
Cheng, Hei Yan; Murdoch, Bruce E.; Goozee, Justine V.; Scott, Dion – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2007
Purpose: This investigation aimed to examine the development of tongue-jaw coordination during speech from childhood to adolescence. Method: Electromagnetic articulography was used to track tongue and jaw motion in 48 children and adults (aged 6-38 years) during productions of /t/ and /k/ embedded in sentences. Results: The coordinative…
Descriptors: Adults, Sentences, Motor Development, Children
Robinson, Christopher W.; Sloutsky, Vladimir M. – Infancy, 2007
Although it is generally accepted that labels facilitate categorization in infancy, recent evidence suggests that infants and young children are more likely to process visual input when presented in isolation than when paired with nonlinguistic sounds or linguistic labels. These findings suggest that auditory input (when compared to a no-auditory…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Linguistics, Infants, Classification
Brand, Rebecca J.; Tapscott, Stephanie – Infancy, 2007
This study investigated whether acoustic input, in the form of infant-directed speech, influenced infants' segmenting of action sequences. Thirty-two 7.5- to 11.5-month-old infants were familiarized with video sequences made up of short action clips. Narration coincided with portions of the action stream to package certain pairs of clips together.…
Descriptors: Infants, Parent Child Relationship, Narration, Acoustics
Quinn, Paul C.; Intraub, Helene – Child Development, 2007
This investigation examined whether infants display "boundary extension"--a tendency to remember more of a visual scene than was presented. Three- to 7-month-olds were familiarized with a photograph of a visual scene, and tested with wide-angle versus close-up views of the scene. Infants preferred the close-up, indicating that they perceived the…
Descriptors: Photography, Infants, Pictorial Stimuli, Visual Perception