NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 20011
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 481 to 495 of 2,359 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mather, Emily; Plunkett, Kim – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
What is the source of the "mutual exclusivity" bias whereby infants map novel labels onto novel objects? In an intermodal preferential looking task, we found that novel labels support 10-month-olds' attention to a novel object over a familiar object. In contrast, familiar labels and a neutral phrase gradually reduced attention to a novel object.…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Infants, Attention, Familiarity
Lattal, Kennon A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
The experimental analysis of delay of reinforcement is considered from the perspective of three questions that seem basic not only to understanding delay of reinforcement but also, by implication, the contributions of temporal relations between events to operant behavior. The first question is whether effects of the temporal relation between…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Operant Conditioning, Delay of Gratification, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Graham, Susan A.; Namy, Laura L.; Gentner, Dedre; Meagher, Kristinn – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
We examined the role of the comparison process and shared names on preschoolers' categorization of novel objects. In our studies, 4-year-olds were presented with novel object sets consisting of either one or two standards and two test objects: a shape match and a texture match. When children were presented with one standard, they extended the…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Classification, Preschool Children, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hanania, Rima – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
In the Dimension Change Card Sort (DCCS) task, 3-year-olds can sort cards well by one dimension but have difficulty in switching to sort the same cards by another dimension when asked; that is, they perseverate on the first relevant information. What is the information that children perseverate on? Using a new version of the DCCS, the experiments…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Stimuli, Task Analysis, Theories
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Giordano, Bruno L.; Rocchesso, Davide; McAdams, Stephen – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
Sound sources are perceived by integrating information from multiple acoustical features. The factors influencing the integration of information are largely unknown. We measured how the perceptual weighting of different features varies with the accuracy of information and with a listener's ability to exploit it. Participants judged the hardness of…
Descriptors: Auditory Perception, Experiments, Experimental Psychology, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Morgan, David L. – Psychological Record, 2010
Published just over a half century ago, "Schedules of Reinforcement" (SOR) (Ferster & Skinner, 1957) marked the seminal empirical contribution of the book's second author and ushered in an era of research on behavior-environment relationships. This article traces the origins of both the methods and the data presented in SOR, and its legacy within…
Descriptors: Books, Behavioral Science Research, Research Methodology, Reinforcement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Carneiro, Paula; Fernandez, Angel – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
Two experiments were conducted to examine whether children of different ages differ in their ability to reject associative false memories with the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. Two different types of manipulations that are thought to facilitate false memory rejection in adults--slowing the presentation rate and issuing explicit…
Descriptors: Models, Editing, Age Differences, Rejection (Psychology)
Michalski, Daniel S.; Cope, Caroline; Fowler, Garth A. – American Psychological Association, 2015
The 2016 Graduate Study in Psychology Summary Report reflects data collected from more than 500 departments and programs offering master's and doctoral degrees in psychology and related training. This report represents data from the 2013-2014 academic year and aggregates these data in the following areas: survey participation; admissions review,…
Descriptors: College Entrance Examinations, Graduate Study, Graduate Students, College Applicants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Simmering, Vanessa R. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The change detection task has been used in dozens of studies with adults to measure visual working memory capacity. Two studies have recently tested children in this task, suggesting a gradual increase in capacity from 5 years to adulthood. These results contrast with findings from an infant looking paradigm suggesting that capacity reaches…
Descriptors: Evidence, Infants, Program Effectiveness, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Johnson, Rebecca L.; Dunne, Maxine D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The current experiments explored the parafoveal processing of transposed-letter (TL) neighbors by using an eye-movement-contingent boundary change paradigm. In Experiment 1 readers received a parafoveal preview of a target word (e.g., "calm") that was either (1) identical to the target word ("calm"), (2) a TL-neighbor ("clam"), or (3) a…
Descriptors: Evidence, Word Recognition, Vocabulary Development, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Otgaar, Henry; Peters, Maarten; Howe, Mark L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
The present study examined the impact of divided attention on children's and adults' neutral and negative true and false memories in a standard Deese/Roediger-McDermott paradigm. Children (7- and 11-year-olds; n = 126) and adults (n = 52) received 5 neutral and 5 negative Deese/Roediger-McDermott word lists; half of each group also received a…
Descriptors: Classroom Techniques, Word Lists, Attention Control, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Waterman, Amanda H.; Blades, Mark – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2011
Adults ask children questions in a variety of contexts, for example, in the classroom, in the forensic context, or in experimental research. In such situations children will inevitably be asked some questions to which they do not know the answer, because they do not have the required information ("unanswerable" questions). When asked unanswerable…
Descriptors: Developmental Psychology, Adults, Children, Questioning Techniques
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zosh, Jennifer M.; Halberda, Justin; Feigenson, Lisa – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2011
The number of individual items that can be maintained in working memory is limited. One solution to this problem is to store representations of ensembles that contain summary information about large numbers of items (e.g., the approximate number or cumulative area of a group of many items). Here we explored the developmental origins of ensemble…
Descriptors: Employment Projections, Infants, Scientific Concepts, Short Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chao, Hsuan-Fu – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
The current study investigated attentional control through active inhibition of the identity of the distractor. Adapting a Stroop paradigm, the distractor word was presented in advance and made to disappear, followed by the presentation of a Stroop stimulus. Participants were instructed to inhibit the distractor in order to reduce its…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Attention Control, Inhibition, Color
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Aslan, Alp; Bauml, Karl-Heinz T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Selectively retrieving a subset of previously studied information enhances memory for the retrieved information but causes forgetting of related, nonretrieved information. Such retrieval-induced forgetting (RIF) has often been attributed to inhibitory executive-control processes that supposedly suppress the nonretrieved items' memory…
Descriptors: Young Adults, Short Term Memory, Recall (Psychology), Correlation
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  29  |  30  |  31  |  32  |  33  |  34  |  35  |  36  |  37  |  ...  |  158