Publication Date
In 2025 | 2 |
Since 2024 | 11 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 39 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 110 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 431 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 178 |
Teachers | 121 |
Students | 21 |
Researchers | 20 |
Support Staff | 4 |
Parents | 3 |
Administrators | 2 |
Community | 1 |
Location
Canada | 6 |
United Kingdom (Great Britain) | 5 |
United States | 5 |
Alaska | 4 |
California | 4 |
United Kingdom | 4 |
Australia | 3 |
Germany | 3 |
North Carolina | 3 |
Sweden | 3 |
United Kingdom (England) | 3 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Elementary and Secondary… | 3 |
United Nations Convention on… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
Attachment Q Set | 3 |
Bayley Scales of Infant… | 1 |
Wechsler Intelligence Scale… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Ludvig, Elliot A.; Staddon, John E. R. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2005
On cyclic-interval reinforcement schedules, animals typically show a postreinforcement pause that is a function of the immediately preceding time interval ("temporal tracking"). Animals, however, do not track single-alternation schedules--when two different intervals are presented in strict alternation on successive trials. In this experiment,…
Descriptors: Animals, Intervals, Reinforcement, Time
Tonneau, Francois; Rios, Americo; Cabrera, Felipe – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
Resistance to change is often studied by measuring response rate in various components of a multiple schedule. Response rate in each component is normalized (that is, divided by its baseline level) and then log-transformed. Differential resistance to change is demonstrated if the normalized, log-transformed response rate in one component decreases…
Descriptors: Behavior Change, Resistance (Psychology), Animal Behavior, Animals
Belke, Terry W.; Duncan, Ian D.; Pierce, W. David – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
Choice between sucrose and wheel-running reinforcement was assessed in two experiments. In the first experiment, ten male Wistar rats were exposed to concurrent VI 30 s VI 30 s schedules of wheel-running and sucrose reinforcement. Sucrose concentration varied across concentrations of 2.5, 7.5, and 12.5%. As concentration increased, more behavior…
Descriptors: Physical Activities, Reinforcement, Behavioral Science Research, Animals
Odum, Amy L.; Ward, Ryan D.; Burke, K. Anne; Barnes, Christopher A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
Four experiments examined the effects of delays to reinforcement on key peck sequences of pigeons maintained under multiple schedules of contingencies that produced variable or repetitive behavior. In Experiments 1, 2, and 4, in the repeat component only the sequence right-right-left-left earned food, and in the vary component four-response…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Animal Behavior, Animals, Behavioral Science Research
Galuska, Chad M.; Winger, Gail; Woods, James H.; Hursh, Steven R. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
Given a commodity available at different prices, a unit-price account of choice predicts preference for the cheaper alternative. This experiment determined if rhesus monkeys preferred remifentanil (an ultra-short-acting [mu]-opioid agonist) delivered at a lower unit price over a higher-priced remifentanil alternative (Phases 1 and 3). Choice…
Descriptors: Prediction, Animals, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research
Mazur, James E. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
Pigeons responded on concurrent-chains schedules with equal variable-interval schedules as initial links. One terminal link delivered a single reinforcer after a fixed delay, and the other terminal link delivered either three or five reinforcers, each preceded by a fixed delay. Some conditions included a postreinforcer delay after the single…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Animals, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research
Young, Michael E.; Beckmann, Joshua S.; Wasserman, Edward A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
We trained four pigeons to discriminate a Michotte launching animation from three other animations using a go/no-go task. The pigeons received food for pecking at one of the animations, but not for pecking at the others. The four animations featured two types of interactions among objects: causal (direct launching) and noncausal (delayed, distal,…
Descriptors: Interaction, Animal Behavior, Animals, Behavioral Science Research
de la Piedad, Xochitl; Field, Douglas; Rachlin, Howard – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
Three pigeons chose between random-interval (RI) and tandem, continuous-reinforcement, fixed-interval (crf-FI) reinforcement schedules by pecking either of two keys. As long as a pigeon pecked on the RI key, both keys remained available. If a pigeon pecked on the crf-FI key, then the RI key became unavailable and the crf-FI timer began to time…
Descriptors: Reinforcement, Intervals, Animals, Animal Behavior
Alsop, Brent; Porritt, Melissa – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
Three pigeons discriminated between two sample stimuli (intensities of red light). The difficulty of the discrimination was varied over four levels. At each level, the relative reinforcer magnitude for the two correct responses was varied across conditions, and the reinforcer rates were equal. Within levels, discriminability between the sample…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Reinforcement, Animals, Animal Behavior
Papachristos, Efstathios B.; Gallistel, C. R. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
In autoshaping experiments, we quantified the acquisition of anticipatory head poking in individual mice, using an algorithm that finds changes in the slope of a cumulative record. In most mice, upward changes in the amount of anticipatory poking per trial were abrupt, and tended to occur at session boundaries, suggesting that the session is as…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Animals, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research
Silverman, Emily; Coffman, Margaret; Younker, Betty – Science and Children, 2007
This article describes an interdisciplinary, activity-based lesson plan implemented in a third/fourth-grade classroom. During these activities, students use musical concepts to think about, illustrate, and discuss animal behavior, and they use scientific concepts to motivate musical composition and performance. The lesson ends with small group…
Descriptors: Scientific Concepts, Musical Composition, Music, Music Teachers
van Duuren, Esther; Nieto Escamez, Francisco A.; Joosten, Ruud N. J. M. A.; Visser, Rein; Mulder, Antonius B.; Pennartz, Cyriel M. A. – Learning & Memory, 2007
The orbitofrontal cortex (OBFc) has been suggested to code the motivational value of environmental stimuli and to use this information for the flexible guidance of goal-directed behavior. To examine whether information regarding reward prediction is quantitatively represented in the rat OBFc, neural activity was recorded during an olfactory…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Rewards, Coding, Knowledge Representation

Kolata, Gina Bari – Science, 1975
Reports on research related to the theory that the nervous systems, and hence the behavior, of animals may be permanently modified during initial periods of early life in response to environmental stimuli. (GS)
Descriptors: Animal Behavior, Biology, Communication Problems, Heredity

Kolata, Gina Bari – Science, 1975
Presents models that ascribe social behavior, such as altruism and parental sacrifices for the young, to a kind of genetic imperative wherein individuals maximize their genetic contribution to the next generation. (GS)
Descriptors: Altruism, Animal Behavior, Biology, Evolution
Long, Sylvester – Weewish Tree, 1975
Cherokee Indian children were named after those animals they had most frequently killed until a chief dreamed that children's names should be derived from the first animal seen at dawn. (JC)
Descriptors: American Indians, Animal Behavior, Childrens Literature, Identification