Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 22 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 122 |
Descriptor
Mental Age | 248 |
Children | 77 |
Autism | 67 |
Comparative Analysis | 65 |
Mental Retardation | 63 |
Down Syndrome | 49 |
Cognitive Development | 37 |
Foreign Countries | 35 |
Adolescents | 34 |
Pervasive Developmental… | 33 |
Age Differences | 31 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Karmiloff-Smith, Annette | 5 |
Dissanayake, Cheryl | 4 |
Loveland, Katherine A. | 4 |
Martin, Gary E. | 4 |
Rogers, Sally J. | 4 |
Cunningham, Cliff | 3 |
Evans, David W. | 3 |
Glenn, Sheila | 3 |
Lewis, Michael | 3 |
Losh, Molly | 3 |
Spitz, Herman H. | 3 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 21 |
Practitioners | 2 |
Location
United Kingdom | 5 |
United Kingdom (England) | 5 |
Australia | 4 |
Netherlands | 4 |
Canada | 3 |
California | 2 |
Illinois | 2 |
Israel | 2 |
Italy | 2 |
North Carolina | 2 |
Arizona | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Dickson, Chata A.; Wang, Sharon S.; Lombard, Kristin M.; Dube, William V. – Research in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2006
Overselective stimulus control was assessed in 29 students at residential schools for individuals with developmental disabilities. Overselectivity testing included three different delayed identity matching-to-sample tasks. Sample stimuli for the Form/Color Test were nine possible combinations of three colors and three forms. On each trial, the S+…
Descriptors: Residential Schools, Special Needs Students, Developmental Disabilities, Testing
Baranek, Grace T.; David, Fabian J.; Poe, Michele D.; Stone, Wendy L.; Watson, Linda R. – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2006
Background: This study describes a new caregiver-report assessment, the Sensory Experiences Questionnaire (SEQ), and explicates the nature of sensory patterns of hyper- and hyporesponsiveness, their prevalence, and developmental correlates in autism relative to comparison groups. Method: Caregivers of 258 children in five diagnostic groups…
Descriptors: Mental Age, Incidence, Autism, Caregivers
Falkman, Kerstin W.; Hjelmquist, Erland – Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2006
A group of non-native, early signing deaf children between the ages of 7 and 11 years were tested on a referential communication task. A group of hearing children matched for sex and mental and chronological age were also included in the study. The aim was to study the deaf children's ability to take another person's perspective in a task that…
Descriptors: Deafness, Hearing (Physiology), Auditory Tests, Matched Groups

Fein, Greta G. – Developmental Psychology, 1973
A four choice discrimination problem was used to examine imitative behavior in children matched in mental age but differing in chronological age. The finding that older children imitated less than younger children is discussed in terms of socialization. (DP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Discrimination Learning, Elementary School Students

Silverstein, A. B.; And Others – Psychology in the Schools, 1981
A series of item analyses of the Concept Assessment Kit Conservation (Goldschmid) conducted for a sample of educable mentally retarded children showed that the probability of a correct response differed from task to task. Evidence indicated that the order of difficulty of the tasks resembled that for nonretarded children. (Author)
Descriptors: Children, Conservation (Concept), Intelligence Quotient, Intelligence Tests
Kelley, Michael F.; And Others – Diagnostique, 1988
Thirty young children with handicaps were assessed on five self-recognition mirror tasks. The set of tasks formed a reproducible scale, indicating that these tasks are an appropriate measure of self-recognition in this population. Data analysis suggested that stage of self-recognition is positively and significantly related to cognitive…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Developmental Stages, Evaluation Methods, Mental Age

Fitzgerald, Hiram E.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
Examined risk factors of 3-year-old sons in 58 families with alcoholic fathers and 16 families with nonalcoholic fathers. Found that sons of alcoholic fathers were more impulsive than sons of nonalcoholic fathers. No differences between the groups in developmental age, IQ, or behavior problems were found. (MDM)
Descriptors: Alcoholism, At Risk Persons, Behavior Problems, Fathers
Glenn, Sheila; Cunningham, Cliff – International Journal of Disability Development and Education, 2004
This study explored the self-understanding of 77 young people with Down syndrome aged from 17 to 24 years, with verbal mental ages ranging from less than 2 years 6 months to 12 years 4 months. The aim was to determine whether the self-understanding of these young people followed a typical developmental pathway, whether they were making social…
Descriptors: Down Syndrome, Young Adults, Self Esteem, Developmental Stages
Shaked, Michal; Yirmiya, Nurit – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2004
In this paper, we summarize some of our findings from a series of three meta-analyses and discuss their implications for autism research. In the first meta-analysis, we examined studies addressing the theory of mind hypothesis in autism. This analysis revealed that theory of mind disabilities are not unique to autism, although what may be unique…
Descriptors: Autism, Meta Analysis, Hypothesis Testing, Siblings
York, J. Steven; And Others – 1977
The paper examines the meaning of transitional objects and attachment to significant others in 18 severely retarded children (mean age 5.2 years). Five case studies illustrate that the use of objects and attachments can be seen as a normal phenomenon and that often the children are responding appropriately to emotional events with behaviors near…
Descriptors: Attachment Behavior, Children, Developmental Stages, Emotional Development

Smith, J. David – American Journal of Mental Deficiency, 1977
Sensitivity to logical contradiction, as manifested by surprise reaction to the Jastrow illusion, was investigated in 30 nonretarded children, 30 educable mentally retarded (EMR) children equated for chronological age, and 30 EMR children equated for mental age. (Author)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Elementary Secondary Education, Exceptional Child Research, Logical Thinking

Brooks-Gunn, Jeanne; Lewis, Michael – Child Development, 1984
Investigates the influence of chronological age, mental age, and handicapping condition on mothers' interactions with their handicapped young children. A total of 111 handicapped children ranging in age from 3 to 36 months and their mothers participated. Infants' general functioning and handicapping conditions were related to maternal…
Descriptors: Cerebral Palsy, Chronological Age, Developmental Disabilities, Disabilities

Quay, Lorene C.; Jarrett, Olga S. – Developmental Psychology, 1984
Evaluates the relative contribution of a number of child characteristics to the prediction of social acceptance. Predictor variables included positive social interactions, occupied behavior when not interacting, mental age, chronological age, and sex. Results supported the efficacy of helping social isolates to interact positively with peers.…
Descriptors: Chronological Age, Individual Characteristics, Mental Age, Peer Relationship

Vandenberg, Brian R. – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1985
Exploratory behavior of educationally mentally retarded 7- to 12-year-old children was compared to that of two independent groups of normal children matched on chronological and mental age. In a multidimensional assessment of exploration, results suggested that delays in exploratory behavior in retarded children are developmental in nature, and…
Descriptors: Behavior Development, Children, Chronological Age, Developmental Disabilities

Rodgers, Joseph Lee – Developmental Psychology, 1984
Applies the confluence model to a longitudinal intact family data set. Also tests the data with simple linear models. Results suggest that the confluence model should be rejected as a useful model for these data based on principles of parsimony and predictability. (Author/AS)
Descriptors: Family Characteristics, Family Influence, Family Structure, Intelligence