Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 12 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 42 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 96 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 247 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Ruff, Holly A. | 6 |
Bekkering, Harold | 5 |
Beck, Sarah R. | 3 |
Bryden, Pamela J. | 3 |
Bub, Daniel N. | 3 |
Glenberg, Arthur M. | 3 |
Gottfried, Allen W. | 3 |
Kunde, Wilfried | 3 |
Levin, Joel R. | 3 |
Li, Weidong | 3 |
Lindemann, Oliver | 3 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 18 |
Teachers | 14 |
Researchers | 13 |
Parents | 5 |
Counselors | 2 |
Students | 1 |
Location
Germany | 7 |
China | 6 |
Netherlands | 5 |
Taiwan | 5 |
Japan | 4 |
United Kingdom | 3 |
Australia | 2 |
France | 2 |
India | 2 |
Indiana | 2 |
Israel | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Pineno, Oskar; de la Casa, Luis Gonzalo; Lubow, R. E.; Miller, Ralph R. – Learning and Motivation, 2006
The present experiments assessed the effects of different manipulations between cue preexposure and cue-outcome pairings on latent inhibition (LI) in a predictive learning task with human participants. To facilitate LI, preexposure and acquisition with the target cues took place while participants performed a secondary task. Presentation of…
Descriptors: Learning Theories, Cues, Inhibition, Experimental Groups
Yung, Hsin I. – ProQuest LLC, 2008
The purpose of this study was to examine the effects of an animated agent that provides instructional scaffolding strategies via a story mnemonic or cuing question with feedback versus instructional scaffolding strategies alone on student achievement of different educational objectives in multimedia learning. Specifically, the study investigated…
Descriptors: Scaffolding (Teaching Technique), Academic Achievement, Educational Strategies, Prompting
Lu, Hongjing; Morrison, Robert G.; Hummel, John E.; Holyoak, Keith J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2006
Previous research has shown that synchronized flicker can facilitate detection of a single Kanizsa square. The present study investigated the role of temporally structured priming in discrimination tasks involving perceptual relations between multiple Kanizsa-type figures. Results indicate that visual information presented as temporally structured…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Geometric Concepts, Visual Perception, Visual Discrimination
Nederveld, Patricia; Thomson, Carole – 1972
The purpose of this sequence of a working paper intended for inclusion in a curriculum manual to be published in the future, is to provide the child with concrete experience, opportunities to represent things symbolically, and to learn the nature of symbols and how to read them. Examining objects, acting them out, and experimenting with them, the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Language Learning Levels, Object Manipulation

Ohwaki, Sonoko; Stayton, Samuel E. – Child Development, 1978
Results indicated that after partialing out CA and MA, number of years in the institution was negatively correlated with success in visually directed object manipulation, social imitation, and purposeful, constructive activities for 103 profoundly retarded residents. (Author/JMB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Environmental Influences, Imitation, Institutionalized Persons

Kelly, Michael H.; Freyd, Jennifer J. – Cognitive Psychology, 1987
Figures that undergo an implied rotation are remembered as being slightly beyond their final position, a phenomenon called representational momentum. Eight experiments explored the questions of what gets transformed and what types of transformations induce such representational distortions. (GDC)
Descriptors: Higher Education, Kinesthetic Perception, Object Manipulation, Schemata (Cognition)

Abravanel, Eugene; Gingold, Herbert – Developmental Psychology, 1985
Deferred imitation of object-related actions was studied at two ages, 12 and 18 months, to examine development of competence in observational learning. Three task categories were investigated: simple/single reiterative, and sequentially coordinated actions. Examination of partial successes at both ages was useful for suggesting phases in…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Early Childhood Education, Imitation, Infants

Fenson, Larry; Schell, Robert E. – Early Child Development and Care, 1985
Discusses developmental changes in visual exploration and manipulative investigation among children from birth to six months, six to 12 months, and 12 to 36 months of age. Also discusses pretend play in terms of decentration, decontextualization, and integration. (RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Exploratory Behavior, Infants, Object Manipulation

Sand, Patricia L.; And Others – American Journal of Occupational Therapy, 1973
Reports on results of the Developmental Hand Function Test administered to 28 educable mentally retarded and 34 normal 12-and 14-year-old girls to show that manual dexterity and functional had skills are compromised in the mentally retarded. (DS)
Descriptors: Intelligence, Mild Mental Retardation, Object Manipulation, Psychomotor Skills

Torrance, E. Paul – Young Children, 1970
An experiment with 66 six-year-old children showed that children asked more and better questions about unfamiliar objects (toys) when they had been given an opportunity to manipulate them than when they saw only demonstrations of the toys. (NH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Kindergarten Children, Object Manipulation, Questioning Techniques

Calder, Clarence R., Jr.; Zalatimo, Suleiman D. – Reading Teacher, 1970
Concludes that direct instruction and practice in following directions increase students' task performance ability and stresses the need to use intrinsically motivating materials in language arts developmental skills programs. Tables and bibliography. (RW)
Descriptors: Material Development, Object Manipulation, Reading Ability, Reading Research

Ruddy, Margaret G.; Bornstein, Marc H. – Child Development, 1982
Investigates the predictability of cognitive differences at 12 months from infant and maternal behaviors at 4 months. Overall, the results show that some individual differences in cognition may be predictable across the first year of life. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Individual Differences, Infant Behavior, Object Manipulation

Laxon, V. J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1981
Sixty children aged 2-3 to 5-6 were given four quantity tasks that tested their understanding of "more" and "same." Tasks involving a manipulative response were significantly easier than those involving a yes/no judgment. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Computation, Concept Formation, Nonverbal Ability, Object Manipulation

Goodman, Sherryl Hope – Child Development, 1981
Results of a study of 38 preschool children observed and videotaped during performance on a jigsaw-puzzle task indicate that puzzle solutions accompanied by a high rate of verbalizations were judged as more proficient, solved with a high rate of puzzle-solving moves, and completed in a shorter period of time. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Child Language, Object Manipulation, Oral Language, Preschool Children

Gottfried, Allen W.; Rose, Susan A. – Child Development, 1980
Twenty-five one-year-olds were administered two tasks (each of which consisted of a familiarization stage followed by a recognition stage) in order to determine whether infants can recognize the shapes of objects by touch alone. (CM)
Descriptors: Developmental Tasks, Infant Behavior, Infants, Memory