Publication Date
In 2025 | 6 |
Since 2024 | 104 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 511 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1490 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 4282 |
Descriptor
Visual Stimuli | 7198 |
Cognitive Processes | 1561 |
Visual Perception | 1332 |
Foreign Countries | 1175 |
Auditory Stimuli | 989 |
Comparative Analysis | 839 |
Cues | 784 |
Infants | 763 |
Attention | 755 |
Age Differences | 731 |
Teaching Methods | 727 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Quinn, Paul C. | 18 |
Smith, Linda B. | 17 |
Humphreys, Glyn W. | 16 |
Johnson, Scott P. | 15 |
Rayner, Keith | 14 |
Colombo, John | 13 |
Pascalis, Olivier | 13 |
Rose, Susan A. | 13 |
Turati, Chiara | 13 |
Bhatt, Ramesh S. | 12 |
Nelson, Charles A. | 12 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 167 |
Teachers | 120 |
Practitioners | 88 |
Parents | 9 |
Students | 3 |
Policymakers | 2 |
Administrators | 1 |
Media Staff | 1 |
Location
United Kingdom | 90 |
Germany | 88 |
Canada | 86 |
Australia | 85 |
China | 59 |
United Kingdom (England) | 55 |
Israel | 50 |
Netherlands | 49 |
California | 44 |
Japan | 42 |
Spain | 36 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards without Reservations | 2 |
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 5 |
Does not meet standards | 2 |
Bradley M. Drysdale; Dennis W. Moore; Brett E. Furlonger; Angelika Anderson – Review Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2018
Despite the increasing number of studies investigating eye gaze patterns in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) during passive viewing of stimuli, few studies have focused on gaze behaviour of people with ASD during active task engagement. Active engagement may cue these individuals to allocate gaze to task-related information and…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Biofeedback, Autism Spectrum Disorders, Performance
Higuchi, Yoko; Ueda, Yoshiyuki; Shibata, Kazuhisa; Saiki, Jun – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
We can incidentally learn regularities in a visual scene, and this kind of learning facilitates subsequent processing of similar scenes. One example of incidental learning is referred to as "contextual cueing," a phenomenon in which repetitive exposure to a particular spatial configuration facilitates visual search performance in the…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Generalization, Cues, Context Effect
Wu, Zhemeng; Kavanova, Martina; Hickman, Lydia; Lin, Fiona; Buckley, Mark J. – Learning & Memory, 2020
According to dual-process theory, recognition memory performance draws upon two processes, familiarity and recollection. The relative contribution to recognition memory are commonly distinguished in humans by analyzing receiver-operating-characteristics (ROC) curves; analogous methods are more complex and very rare in animals but fast familiarity…
Descriptors: Memory, Recognition (Psychology), Familiarity, Recall (Psychology)
Kadooka, Kellan; Franchak, John M. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Visual attention in complex, dynamic scenes is attracted to locations that contain socially relevant features, such as faces, and to areas that are visually salient. Previous work suggests that there is a "global shift" over development such that observers increasingly attend to faces with age. However, no prior work has tested whether…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Development, Human Body, Visual Stimuli
Mazumdar, Barnali; Donovan, Neila J.; Sultana, Asifa – International Journal of Language & Communication Disorders, 2020
Background: A comprehensive aphasia assessment is necessary to diagnose the type and severity of aphasia differentially and guide appropriate interventions. One component of an aphasia assessment is the picture description task (PDT), designed to probe spontaneous speech fluency and information content. Most aphasia assessments use black-and-white…
Descriptors: Aphasia, Communication Disorders, Clinical Diagnosis, Pictorial Stimuli
Visual Disengagement: Genetic Architecture and Relation to Autistic Traits in the General Population
Siqueiros Sanchez, Monica; Pettersson, Erik; Kennedy, Daniel P.; Bölte, Sven; Lichtenstein, Paul; D'Onofrio, Brian M.; Falck-Ytter, Terje – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2020
Visual disengagement has been hypothesized as an endophenotype for autism. In this study we used twin modelling to assess the role of genetics in basic measures of visual disengagement, and tested their putative association to autistic traits in the general population. We used the Gap Overlap task in a sample of 492 twins. Results showed that most…
Descriptors: Genetics, Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Symptoms (Individual Disorders)
Mazzocco, Michèle M. M.; Chan, Jenny Yun-Chen; Bye, Jeffrey K.; Padrutt, Emily R.; Praus-Singh, Taylor; Lukowski, Sarah; Brown, Ethan; Olson, Rachel E. – Mathematical Thinking and Learning: An International Journal, 2020
Children's spontaneous focus on numerosity (SFON) is described as an unprompted tendency that is stable across contexts. The attention to number task (AtN), an experimental forced-choice picture-matching task designed to evaluate select aspects of children's focus on numerosity, may reveal whether task materials can implicitly prompt children to…
Descriptors: Numeracy, Mathematics Skills, Adults, Attention
Hupp, Julie M.; Jungers, Melissa K.; Porter, Brandon L.; Plunkett, Brandy A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2020
When hearing an object label, a specific object may come to mind. With the phrase, "There was a balloon in the pack/air" the representation of balloon varies based on the implied shape (deflated vs. inflated). The current study investigated whether the implied shape affects sentence-picture verification for adults and preschool children.…
Descriptors: Adults, Preschool Children, Age Differences, Sentences
Krueger, Breanna I.; Storkel, Holly L. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2020
Purpose: The purpose of this study was to examine whether children's identification of misarticulated words as real objects was influenced by an inherent bias toward selecting real objects or whether a change in experimental conditions could impact children's selections. Method: Forty preschool children aged 4 years 0 months to 6 years 11 months…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Articulation (Speech), Word Recognition, Responses
Hanna, Joy E.; Brennan, Susan E.; Savietta, Kelly J. – Discourse Processes: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2020
During face-to-face communication, people use visual cues about what their partners are attending to as they process language. An eyetracking experiment explored how addressees use speakers' eye gaze and head orientation while interpreting references to objects in a spatial task. Thirty-six naive director/matcher pairs seated face-to-face were…
Descriptors: Eye Movements, Visual Stimuli, Cues, Interpersonal Communication
Ruba, Ashley L.; Meltzoff, Andrew N.; Repacholi, Betty M. – Developmental Psychology, 2020
Accurate perception of emotional (facial) expressions is an essential social skill. It is currently debated whether emotion categorization in infancy emerges in a "broad-to-narrow" pattern and the degree to which language influences this process. We used an habituation paradigm to explore (a) whether 14- and 18-month-old infants perceive…
Descriptors: Infants, Nonverbal Communication, Emotional Response, Toddlers
Lee, Hongmi; Kim, Kyungmi; Yi, Do-Joon – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Previous studies have reported contradictory findings regarding the effects of item repetition on the subsequent encoding of contextual details associated with items (i.e., source memory). Whereas some studies reported repetition-induced enhancement in source memory, other studies observed repetition-induced impairment. To resolve these…
Descriptors: Memory, Familiarity, Context Effect, Tests
Offerman, Heather M. – ProQuest LLC, 2020
Having historically received less attention than other linguistic structures (Derwing & Munro, 2005), second language (L2) pronunciation instruction represents an emergent area of research in the field (Thomson & Derwing, 2015). While several methods have been shown to be effective for improving L2 segmental production, including explicit…
Descriptors: Second Language Instruction, Pronunciation, Direct Instruction, Visual Stimuli
Castelhano, Monica S.; Fernandes, Suzette; Theriault, Jordan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
How are scene representations stored in memory? Researchers have often posited that scene representations have a hierarchical structure with background elements providing a scaffold for more detailed foreground elements. To further investigate scene representation and the role of background and foreground information, we introduced a new stimulus…
Descriptors: Memory, Visual Perception, Visual Stimuli, Context Effect
Wiese, Holger; Chan, Chelsea Y. X.; Tüttenberg, Simone C. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2019
It is difficult to recognize the identity of a face presented in negative contrast. This difficulty, however, is substantially reduced when only the eye region is contrast positive in an otherwise negative face image, and recognition of these so-called contrast chimeras approaches performance with full positive faces. This apparently similar…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Recognition (Psychology), Human Body, Identification