NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 2,146 to 2,160 of 2,477 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fletcher, Paul; Leonard, Laurence B.; Stokes, Stephanie F.; Wong, Anita M.-Y. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2005
Previous studies of verb morphology in children with specific language impairment (SLI) have been limited in the main to tense and agreement morphemes. Cantonese, which, like other Chinese languages, has no grammatical tense, presents an opportunity to investigate potential difficulties for children with SLI in other areas of verb morphology, via…
Descriptors: Verbs, Morphemes, Language Impairments, Sino Tibetan Languages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Idrissi, Ali; Kehayia, Eva – Brain and Language, 2004
An ongoing debate in Arabic morphology concerns the nature of the smallest unit governing lexical organization and representation in this language. A standard model maintains that Arabic words are typically analyzable into a three-consonantal root morpheme carrying the core meaning of words and a prosodic template responsible mostly for…
Descriptors: Morphology (Languages), Semitic Languages, Dyslexia, Linguistic Theory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Liow, Susan J. Rickard; Lee, Lay Choo – Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2004
The Malay language has a transparent morphological system and, unlike English, it is written in a very shallow alphabetic-syllabic script. We predicted that beginner spellers (six-to eight-year-olds) of this Rumi script would encode words at the level of the syllable and morpheme, rather than the phoneme. Using the results of a 75-item spelling…
Descriptors: Metalinguistics, Spelling, Indonesian Languages, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Frost, Ram; Kugler, Tamar; Deutsch, Avital; Forster, Kenneth I. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Most models of visual word recognition in alphabetic orthographies assume that words are lexically organized according to orthographic similarity. Support for this is provided by form-priming experiments that demonstrate robust facilitation when primes and targets share similar sequences of letters. The authors examined form-orthographic priming…
Descriptors: Semitic Languages, English, Contrastive Linguistics, Morphology (Languages)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Tyler, Ann A.; Lewis, Kerry E.; Haskill, Allison; Tolbert, Leslie C. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2003
The purpose of this study was to assess phonological and morphosyntactic change in children with co-occurring speech and language impairments using different goal attack strategies. Participants included 47 preschoolers, ages 3;0 (years;months) to 5;11, with impairments in both speech and language: 40 children in the experimental group and 7 in a…
Descriptors: Experimental Groups, Control Groups, Intervention, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Canagarajah, A. Suresh – International Multilingual Research Journal, 2007
Global English is under contestation. Although some consider lingua franca English (LFE) as a neutral medium or code that does not belong to any specific culture or nationality, others see the deceptive nature of this linguistic globalization. Along with Spring (2007/this issue), they see global English as embodying partisan interests and values.…
Descriptors: Linguistic Borrowing, Morphemes, Multilingualism, Global Approach
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lemhofer, Kristin; Schriefers, Herbert; Jescheniak, Jorg D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
In many languages, the production of noun phrases requires the selection of gender-marked elements like determiners or inflectional suffixes. There is a recent debate as to whether the selection of freestanding gender-marked elements, such as determiners, follows the same processing mechanisms as the selection of bound gender-marked morphemes,…
Descriptors: Uncommonly Taught Languages, Indo European Languages, Morphemes, Suffixes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Martin, Amber Joy; Sera, Maria D. – Journal of Deaf Studies and Deaf Education, 2006
Spatial relations in American Sign Language (ASL) are often signed from the perspective of the signer and so involve a shift in perspective and mental rotation. This study examined developing knowledge of language used to refer to the spatial relations "front," "behind," "left," "right," "towards," "away," "above," and "below" by children learning…
Descriptors: Language Acquisition, Spatial Ability, American Sign Language, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carter, Anne L. – Journal of Child Language, 1975
Through discussion and illustrative events, an evolving segment of communication is described during the course of transition of one child's total communication system from the sensorimotor or gestural level at 12 months into the level of use of the adult words "more" and "mine," and associated utterances, at 24 months. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Development, Child Language, Infant Behavior, Language Acquisition
Jager, Gert – Deutsch als Fremdsprache, 1974
Paper presented May 1973, Dresden, East Germany. (DD)
Descriptors: Grammar, Instructional Materials, Language Instruction, Linguistic Competence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
MacWhinney, Brian – Journal of Child Language, 1975
This study examines the relative contributions of rote-memorization, analogic formation and rule-operation in the production of plurals. Rule-operation was found to be important in that children producing responses characteristic of a given stage did not produce responses for later stages. Contributions of analogic formation and rote-memorization…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation, Language Acquisition
Cousins, Andrea – 1979
Major findings are reported of a longitudinal, naturalistic study of grammatical morpheme development in an aphasic child from 5;5 to 6;1. The majority of the morphemes were not acquired in the same order nor at the same mean length of utterance (MLU) levels reported for normal children. As an alternative to the normal acquisition model, based on…
Descriptors: Aphasia, Child Language, Cognitive Development, Language Acquisition
Goodman, Kenneth S.; Burke, Carolyn L. – 1968
Psycholinguistic knowledge and techniques can be used as the basis for reading process research with a view to the development of a reading theory. The initial phase of such a study is reported, and reading behavior on a comprehensive basis is examined. The assumption upon which the research is based is that reading miscues are generated by the…
Descriptors: Classification, Grammar, Intermediate Grades, Morphemes
Olsen, H.C. – 1968
The use of linguistic principles in selecting reading materials is relatively recent. Several years ago at Wayne State University, over 200 principles were identified as possible criteria for the selection of materials. About 50 of these were linguistic principles, some of which conflicted with the others. Descriptive linguistics seems, at…
Descriptors: Criteria, Graphemes, Individual Differences, Instructional Materials
Whitaker, Harry A. – 1968
The tone-changing rules in Central (Standard) Thai are traditionally analyzed as having five contrasting tones--mid, low, high, falling, and rising. (Abramson's graph of the fundamental frequency variations for single vowel Thai syllables illustrates these five tones.) Theoretically, each tone may be part of any Thai syllable. Any Thai syllable…
Descriptors: Descriptive Linguistics, Distinctive Features (Language), Morphemes, Phonology
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  140  |  141  |  142  |  143  |  144  |  145  |  146  |  147  |  148  |  ...  |  166