NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Individuals with Disabilities…1
Showing 541 to 555 of 7,193 results Save | Export
Frey, Nancy; Fisher, Douglas; Smith, Dominique – Educational Leadership, 2020
Color. Carpets. Configurations. The classroom can create a sense of safety, calm, and invitation to learn if designed correctly. Authors and educators Nancy Frey, Douglas Fisher, and Dominique Smith describe their journey to making their school's classrooms trauma-informed and safe, inviting spaces for students.
Descriptors: Trauma, Classroom Environment, Student Needs, At Risk Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ferry, Alissa; Nespor, Marina; Mehler, Jacques – Developmental Psychology, 2020
To learn a language infants must learn to link arbitrary sounds to their meaning. While words are the clearest example of this link, they are not the only component of language; morphological regularities (e.g., the plural -s suffix in English) carry meaning as well. Comprehensive theories of language acquisition must account for how infants build…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Language, Comprehension, Morphology (Languages)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schulz, Samantha E.; Stevenson, Ryan A. – Autism: The International Journal of Research and Practice, 2020
Recent studies have suggested that individuals who exhibit heightened sensitivity also exhibit higher rates and severity of restricted interests and repetitive behaviours. This line of research has been conducted almost exclusively through caregiver reports of sensitivity. Here, a more rigorous psychophysics paradigm was applied to assess sensory…
Descriptors: Behavior Problems, Repetition, Sensory Experience, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Derakhshanrad, Seyed Alireza; Piven, Emily – International Journal of Developmental Disabilities, 2020
Objective: This retrospective case study of an eight year old female with autism illuminates how alteration of the training environment using florescent objects under blacklight conditions, may have been the facilitating impetus that increased her interaction with objects over time. Methods: This study approach was chosen to best correspond with…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Light, Environmental Influences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Matell, Matthew S.; Della Valle, Rebecca B. – Learning & Memory, 2018
Presentation of a previously trained Pavlovian conditioned stimulus while an organism is engaged in operant responding can moderate the rate of responding, a phenomenon known as Pavlovian-to-instrumental transfer. Although it is well known that Pavlovian contingencies will generate conditioned behavior that is temporally organized with respect to…
Descriptors: Operant Conditioning, Experiments, Animals, Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stavans, Maayan; Baillargeon, Renée – Developmental Science, 2018
Two experiments examined whether 4-month-olds (n = 120) who were induced to assign two objects to different categories would then be able to take advantage of these contrastive categorical encodings to individuate and track the objects. In each experiment, infants first watched functional demonstrations of two tools, a masher and tongs (Experiment…
Descriptors: Infants, Eye Movements, Visual Stimuli, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Minar, Nicholas J.; Lewkowicz, David J. – Developmental Science, 2018
We tested 4-6- and 10-12-month-old infants to investigate whether the often-reported decline in infant sensitivity to other-race faces may reflect responsiveness to static or dynamic/silent faces rather than a general process of perceptual narrowing. Across three experiments, we tested discrimination of either dynamic own-race or other-race faces…
Descriptors: Infants, Age Differences, Attention, Visual Discrimination
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kocab, Annemarie; Lam, Hannah; Snedeker, Jesse – Cognitive Science, 2018
A well-known typological observation is the dominance of subject-initial word orders, SOV and SVO, across the world's languages. Recent findings from gestural language creation paradigms offer possible explanations for the prevalence of SOV. When asked to gesture transitive events with an animate agent and inanimate patient, gesturers tend to…
Descriptors: Word Order, Nonverbal Communication, Hypothesis Testing, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ganesh, Attigodu Chandrashekara; Berthommier, Frédéric; Schwartz, Jean-Luc – Language Learning, 2018
Speech perception involves fusion of multiple sensory inputs, but fusion is not automatic, likely depending on several external and internal factors (e.g., attention, noise, age). In this study, we exploited a specific paradigm in which a short audiovisual context made of coherent or incoherent speech material is displayed before an incongruent…
Descriptors: Aging (Individuals), Acoustics, Speech Communication, Auditory Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Atlas, Lauren Y.; Phelps, Elizabeth A. – Learning & Memory, 2018
Fear-relevant stimuli such as snakes and spiders are thought to capture attention due to evolutionary significance. Classical conditioning experiments indicate that these stimuli accelerate learning, while instructed extinction experiments suggest they may be less responsive to instructions. We manipulated stimulus type during instructed aversive…
Descriptors: Fear, Stimuli, Hypothesis Testing, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Westerberg, Carmen E.; Hawkins, Christopher A.; Rendon, Lauren – Learning & Memory, 2018
Reality-monitoring errors occur when internally generated thoughts are remembered as external occurrences. We hypothesized that sleep-dependent memory consolidation could reduce them by strengthening connections between items and their contexts during an afternoon nap. Participants viewed words and imagined their referents. Pictures of the…
Descriptors: Sleep, Memory, Hypothesis Testing, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rollins, Leslie; Khuu, Alexis; Lodi, Nafeesa – Learning & Memory, 2019
On forced-choice tests of recognition memory, performance is best when targets are paired with novel foils (A-X), followed by corresponding lures (A-A'), and then noncorresponding lures (A-B'). The current study tested the prediction that encoding variability accounts for reduced performance on A-B' trials. Young adults (n = 43) completed the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Recognition (Psychology), Memory, Young Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Neely, Kristina A.; Mohanty, Suman; Schmitt, Lauren M.; Wang, Zheng; Sweeney, John A.; Mosconi, Matthew W. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2019
Sensorimotor abnormalities are common in individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD); however, the processes underlying these deficits remain unclear. This study examined force production with and without visual feedback to determine if individuals with ASD can utilize internal representations to guide sustained force. Individuals with ASD…
Descriptors: Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Feedback (Response), Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hanney, Nicole M.; Carr, James E.; LeBlanc, Linda A. – Journal of Applied Behavior Analysis, 2019
Studies on teaching tacts to individuals with autism spectrum disorder (ASD) have primarily focused on visual stimuli, despite published clinical recommendations to teach tacts of stimuli in other sensory domains as well. In the current study, two children with ASD were taught to tact auditory stimuli under two stimulus-presentation arrangements:…
Descriptors: Teaching Methods, Autism, Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lalonde, Kaylah; Werner, Lynne A. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2019
Purpose: This study assessed the extent to which 6- to 8.5-month-old infants and 18- to 30-year-old adults detect and discriminate auditory syllables in noise better in the presence of visual speech than in auditory-only conditions. In addition, we examined whether visual cues to the onset and offset of the auditory signal account for this…
Descriptors: Infants, Young Adults, Auditory Discrimination, Auditory Perception
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  33  |  34  |  35  |  36  |  37  |  38  |  39  |  40  |  41  |  ...  |  480