Publication Date
In 2025 | 6 |
Since 2024 | 120 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 502 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1203 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 2543 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 125 |
Teachers | 91 |
Students | 38 |
Researchers | 32 |
Parents | 2 |
Support Staff | 1 |
Location
China | 68 |
Canada | 60 |
Turkey | 60 |
Japan | 53 |
Germany | 49 |
United Kingdom | 40 |
Spain | 36 |
Australia | 34 |
Netherlands | 30 |
Thailand | 29 |
United Kingdom (England) | 28 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
National Defense Education… | 9 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards | 4 |

Mollica, Anthony – Mosaic: A Journal for Language Teachers, 1995
Presents some suggestions for the writing of poetry to expand vocabulary in the target language. Since poetry demands preciseness, writing poetically helps students develop this language skill. (Author/CK)
Descriptors: Adjectives, Bulletin Boards, Class Activities, Classroom Techniques

Forbes, James N.; Farrar, M. Jeffrey – Cognitive Development, 1995
Systematically explored how three different initial training contexts affect children's and adults' interpretation of novel action verbs. Subjects included 54 3-yearolds, 60 10-year olds, and 60 college-age adults. Findings suggest a hierarchy of verb learning strategies, especially among the youngest children. (DR)
Descriptors: Age Differences, College Students, Context Effect, Language Acquisition

Falmagne, Rachel Joffe; And Others – Cognitive Development, 1994
Investigated third and sixth graders' understanding of factive presupposition using two tasks: one requiring an abstract truth judgment of the verb complement, the other calling for informal judgment of consistency between the target sentence and the negation of its complement. Results indicated the development of factive presupposition is an…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Elementary Education, Elementary School Students, Grade 3

Watzinger-Tharp, Johanna – Foreign Language Annals, 1994
Offers an analysis of native speech data as a base for teaching the past tense in German. Interviews with native speakers revealed that the present perfect serves as the dominant past tense for main verbs; modal verbs, the copula "sein,""haben," and certain formulaic expressions occur in the preterite; and past tense use is linked to contextual…
Descriptors: Databases, German, Grammar, Interviews

Hoff-Ginsberg, Erika; Naigles, Letitia R. – Journal of Child Language, 1998
A study investigated the extent to which the nature of verb input accounts for the order in which children acquire verbs. Subjects were 57 mothers and their Stage I children. Results suggest that the effect of syntactic diversity in input supports the "syntactic bootstrapping" account of how children use structural information to learn new verbs'…
Descriptors: Child Language, Interpersonal Communication, Language Acquisition, Language Processing

Schwanenflugel, Paula J.; Henderson, Robbie L.; Fabricius, William V. – Developmental Psychology, 1998
Assessed developments in the theory of mind suggested by changes in the organization of cognitive verb extensions during elementary school years. Found three major changes with development: increased understanding of the role of memory in input functions, increased interrelatedness of memory- and comprehension-related verbs, and increased…
Descriptors: Adults, Age Differences, Child Development, Classification

Nishiyama, Kunio – Journal of East Asian Linguistics, 1998
Argues for a fundamental structural similarity between serial verb constructions, widely known from Kwa languages, and v-v compounds in Japanese. A major theoretical implication of the analysis is that it supports an analysis of clausal structure where the external argument is not included in the immediate projection of a verb but is introduced by…
Descriptors: African Languages, Contrastive Linguistics, Foreign Countries, Grammar
Park, Kyung-Ja; Nakano, Michiko – Journal of Japan-Korea Association of Applied Linguistics, 1998
This investigation encompassed a full-scale experiment for both Japanese and Korean university students and looked at the following: the role of grammaticality-judgment tasks (GJT) in second-language acquisition; the learners' behaviors when they were asked to do GJT; and the reasons why the learners made the wrong grammatical judgments.…
Descriptors: College Students, Error Analysis (Language), Error Patterns, Foreign Countries

Shook, David J. – Applied Language Learning, 1999
Provides information regarding the input-to-intake phenomenon by exploring data that were gathered but not analyzed in Shook (1994): reading recalls produced by subjects after reading the input passages. Two levels of Spanish students read reading passages containing one of two different target items under one of three different attention…
Descriptors: College Students, Higher Education, Language Tests, Linguistic Input

Juffs, Alan – Language Learning, 1998
Investigated how adult learners of English-as-a-Second- Language (ESL) process sentences containing verbs that are temporarily ambiguous in interpretation between a main verb and a reduced relative clause. Seventeen Chinese, 17 Korean or Japanese, and 17 Romance learners with advanced ESL proficiency and a comparison group of 17 monolingual native…
Descriptors: Adults, Advanced Students, Ambiguity, Comparative Analysis

Plunkett, Kim; Nakisa, Ramin Charles – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1997
Connectionist accounts of inflectional morphology have focused on domain of English past tense in which default process (add/ed) reflects process of suffixation adopted by majority of forms in the language. Arabic plural system is one where a minority default process operates. The study contrasts two types of default process that might lead to a…
Descriptors: Arabic, Contrastive Linguistics, English, Form Classes (Languages)

Franco, Jon; Landa, Alazne – Southwest Journal of Linguistics, 1998
Basque auxiliary verbs encode tense, agreement relations with ergative, absolutive, and dative arguments, which constitute an inflectional verbal amalgam whose acquisition is not problematic for Spanish-speaking children but is for Spanish-speaking adults. This asymmetry is due to different processes by which the inflectional amalgam is acquired.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Basque, Child Language, Comparative Analysis

Montrul, Silvina – Second Language Research, 2001
Presents three related experiments on the acquisition of two classes of causative verbs: physical change of state verbs with agentive subjects and psychological change of state verbs with experiencer objects in English, Spanish, and Turkish as second languages by speakers whose native languages are English, Spanish, Turkish, and Japanese.…
Descriptors: English, English (Second Language), Japanese, Language Acquisition
Lennon, Paul – IRAL, 1996
Focuses on the errors made in lexical verb choice in speech by a small group of advanced learners of English. Findings indicate that while these learners may have a broad outline of verb meaning, their knowledge is hazy concerning contextual and collocational restrictions. Advanced learners may require detailed classroom vocabulary work on simple…
Descriptors: Advanced Courses, College Students, Databases, English (Second Language)

Juffs, Alan – Second Language Research, 1996
Examines knowledge of semantics-syntax correspondences in second-language acquisition (SLA) within the Principles and Parameters framework. A parameter of semantic structure is used to investigate change of state locatives and "psychological" verbs. Results indicate that for some learners, first-language influence persists until advanced stages of…
Descriptors: Adult Students, Chinese, Dictionaries, English (Second Language)