Publication Date
In 2025 | 5 |
Since 2024 | 95 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 481 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1328 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3232 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
DiPerna, Paul | 13 |
Brainerd, C. J. | 10 |
Pascalis, Olivier | 9 |
Yonelinas, Andrew P. | 9 |
Moscovitch, Morris | 8 |
Tomasello, Michael | 8 |
Crossley, Scott A. | 7 |
Merriman, William E. | 7 |
Oakes, Lisa M. | 7 |
Reyna, V. F. | 7 |
Wixted, John T. | 7 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Teachers | 50 |
Researchers | 16 |
Practitioners | 7 |
Counselors | 6 |
Media Staff | 6 |
Students | 5 |
Policymakers | 4 |
Parents | 3 |
Administrators | 2 |
Community | 2 |
Support Staff | 1 |
More ▼ |
Location
Australia | 101 |
Turkey | 89 |
Canada | 72 |
United Kingdom | 70 |
China | 62 |
United States | 54 |
California | 48 |
United Kingdom (England) | 47 |
Germany | 40 |
Iran | 39 |
Netherlands | 37 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Brooks, Nancy J.; Solloway, Sharon G.; Allen, Louise Anderson – Journal of Personnel Evaluation in Education, 2007
The gulf between educational leadership theory and contemporary curriculum scholarship is becoming increasingly problematic now that principals have been legally mandated to add curriculum monitoring to their duties as instructional leaders. Lacking familiarity with curriculum theory and practice, many overburdened administrators are turning to…
Descriptors: Curriculum Research, Familiarity, Instructional Leadership, Principals
Conderman, Greg; Elf, Nanci – Reading & Writing Quarterly, 2007
One of the challenges of providing students with disabilities access to the general education curriculum is their skill in using grade-level materials. Due to their expository nature and specialized vocabulary, textbooks in particular pose challenges for students. This article presents an activity that promotes familiarity with textbook structures…
Descriptors: Textbooks, Familiarity, Reading Difficulties, Secondary Education
Miyakoshi, Makoto; Nomura, Michio; Ohira, Hideki – Brain and Cognition, 2007
We performed an event-related potential study to investigate the self-relevance effect in object recognition. Three stimulus categories were prepared: SELF (participant's own objects), FAMILIAR (disposable and public objects, defined as objects with less-self-relevant familiarity), and UNFAMILIAR (others' objects). The participants' task was to…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Recognition (Psychology), Visual Stimuli, Task Analysis
Schmidt, Gwen L.; DeBuse, Casey J.; Seger, Carol A. – Brain and Language, 2007
Previous laterality studies have implicated the right hemisphere in the processing of metaphors, however it is not clear if this result is due to metaphoricity per se or another aspect of semantic processing. Three divided visual field experiments varied metaphorical and literal sentence familiarity. We found a right hemisphere advantage for…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Familiarity, Sentences, Semantics
Tunnicliffe, Sue Dale; Gatt, Suzanne; Agius, Catherine; Pizzuto, Sue Anne – EURASIA Journal of Mathematics, Science & Technology Education, 2008
Young Maltese children have experience and knowledge of animals. We explored the range of animal with which they are familiar and the origin of this knowledge. The children interviewed were in Pre School, aged 4 years, and in the first year of compulsory education, aged 5 years Verb l questions and photographs were used as the probe to access…
Descriptors: Animals, Visual Aids, Foreign Countries, Young Children
Templeton, Thomas Clay – Library Quarterly, 2008
Place reminds us to tailor our professional work to the shifting concerns, and indeed the shifting criteria, of our shifting constituencies, rather than to a placeless professional vision of what librarianship, society, and literacy should be. Every place maker must make do, make sense, and make place in an environment that is technologically…
Descriptors: Constructivism (Learning), Human Geography, Familiarity, Phenomenology
Merriman, William E.; Lipko, Amanda R. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Preschool-age children were hypothesized to use one of two criteria, cue recognition or target generation, to make several linguistic judgments. When deciding whether a word is one they know, for example, some were expected to consider whether they recognized its sound form (cue recognition), whereas others were expected to consider whether a…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Metalinguistics, Semantics, Familiarity
Thonn, Jessica A. – Language Learning Journal, 2008
Older students (aged 65+) in the language classroom may be presenting hearing deficits not experienced by their traditional university classmates (aged 18-24). L2 teachers need to be aware of the main processes involved in hearing, as well as such important factors as the temporary threshold effect and masking effects, in order to encourage…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Phonemes, Hearing Impairments, Familiarity
Teo, Timothy; Hargreaves, David J.; Lee, June – Journal of Research in Music Education, 2008
The authors investigate whether there were significant differences in preferences for, familiarity with, and identification of Chinese, Malay, and Indian music between adolescent students from Singapore (n = 78) and the United Kingdom (n = 53). Also explored are the relationships among these three variables. Students were asked to rate their…
Descriptors: Music, Females, Familiarity, Identification
Gregg, Melissa K.; Samuel, Arthur G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2008
Change blindness, or the failure to detect (often large) changes to visual scenes, has been demonstrated in a variety of different situations. Failures to detect auditory changes are far less studied, and thus little is known about the nature of change deafness. Five experiments were conducted to explore the processes involved in change deafness…
Descriptors: Cues, Familiarity, Infants, Auditory Perception
Lam, Kristen S. L.; Bodfish, James W.; Piven, Joseph – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2008
Background: Restricted repetitive behaviors (RRBs) are a core feature of autism and consist of a variety of behaviors, ranging from motor stereotypies to complex circumscribed interests. The objective of the current study was to examine the structure of RRBs in autism using relevant items from the Autism Diagnostic Interview-Revised in a sample of…
Descriptors: Autism, Behavior Patterns, Factor Analysis, Symptoms (Individual Disorders)
Hedrick, Wanda B.; Harmon, Janis M.; Wood, Karen – Reading Psychology, 2008
This investigation focused on two research questions. The first question examined what specific instructional strategies for teaching vocabulary appear most frequently in current content area textbooks. The second question built on the first and used that information to develop a survey that was administered to secondary-level preservice teachers…
Descriptors: Educational Strategies, Preservice Teachers, Textbooks, Semantics
Bauman, Sheri – Journal of Early Adolescence, 2010
Students (N = 221) in an intermediate school (grades 5-8) in a rural area of the Southwestern United States completed a survey regarding their familiarity with technology and their experiences with cyberbullying during the school year. Initial evidence of survey reliability is presented. In the sample, 1.5% of participants were classified as…
Descriptors: Student Attitudes, Familiarity, Rural Areas, Computer Mediated Communication
Learning Words: Children Disregard Some Pragmatic Information that Conflicts with Mutual Exclusivity
Jaswal, Vikram K.; Hansen, Mikkel B. – Developmental Science, 2006
Children tend to infer that when a speaker uses a new label, the label refers to an unlabeled object rather than one they already know the label for. Does this inference reflect a default assumption that words are mutually exclusive? Or does it instead reflect the result of a pragmatic reasoning process about what the speaker intended? In two…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Pragmatics, Semantics, Cues
Clearfield, Melissa W.; Westfahl, Shannon May-Comyns – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2006
Previous research suggests that infants can calculate simple arithmetic (Wynn, 1992). This study sought to replicate this finding and examine how familiarization may influence response. Experiment 1 confirmed that 3- to 5-month-old infants looked longer at an incorrect outcome (1 + 1 = 1) when no familiarization trials were presented. Experiment 2…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Processes, Arithmetic, Familiarity