NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 361 to 375 of 1,276 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hills, Thomas T. – Cognitive Science, 2006
Foraging-and feeding-related behaviors across eumetazoans share similar molecular mechanisms, suggesting the early evolution of an optimal foraging behavior called area-restricted search (ARS), involving mechanisms of dopamine and glutamate in the modulation of behavioral focus. Similar mechanisms in the vertebrate basal ganglia control motor…
Descriptors: Animals, Animal Behavior, Evolution, Neurological Organization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bjorklund, David F. – Developmental Review, 2006
Contemporary evolution biology has recognized the role of development in evolution. Evolutionarily oriented psychologists have similarly recognized the role that behavioral plasticity, particularly early in development, may have had on the evolution of species, harking back to the ideas of Baldwin (the Baldwin effect). Epigenetic theories of…
Descriptors: Mothers, Genetics, Evolution, Intelligence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Suda, Chikako; Call, Josep – Cognition, 2006
The study investigates what an intermediate success rate means in bonobos, chimpanzees, and orangutans. Apes participated in liquid conservation experiments where they had to track the larger of two different quantities of juice after various kinds of transformations [Suda, C., & Call, J. (2004). Piagetian liquid conservation in the great apes…
Descriptors: Primatology, Success, Animal Behavior, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Madden, Gregory J.; Smethells, John R.; Ewan, Eric E.; Hursh, Steven R. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
This experiment was conducted to test the predictions of two behavioral-economic approaches to quantifying relative reinforcer efficacy. The normalized demand analysis suggests that characteristics of averaged normalized demand curves may be used to predict progressive-ratio breakpoints and peak responding. By contrast, the demand analysis holds…
Descriptors: Positive Reinforcement, Correlation, Prediction, Evaluation Methods
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Amundson, Jeffrey C.; Miller, Ralph R. – Learning and Motivation, 2007
Two lick suppression studies were conducted with water-deprived rats to investigate the influence of spatial similarity in cue interaction. Experiment 1 assessed the influence of similarity of the spatial origin of competing cues in a blocking procedure. Greater blocking was observed in the condition in which the auditory blocking cue and the…
Descriptors: Inhibition, Spatial Ability, Cues, Competition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Darling, Randi A. – Bioscene: Journal of College Biology Teaching, 2007
This field exercise examines the feeding preferences of Gray Squirrels ("Sciurus carolinensis"). Students present squirrels with a variety of food types in a cafeteria-style arrangement in order to test hypotheses about foraging preferences. This exercise, which is appropriate for introductory biology, ecology, and animal behavior classes, is…
Descriptors: Animal Behavior, Ecology, Science Instruction, Food
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Soto, Florentina; Giese, K. Peter; Edwards, Frances A.; Parsley, Stephanie L.; Pilgram, Sara M. – Learning & Memory, 2007
[alpha]CaMKII[superscript T286A] mutant mice lack long-term potentiation (LTP) in the hippocampal CA1 region and are impaired in spatial learning. In situ hybridization confirms that the mutant mice show the same developmental expression of [alpha]CaMKII as their wild-type littermates. A simple hypothesis would suggest that if LTP is a substrate…
Descriptors: Hypothesis Testing, Neuropsychology, Animal Behavior, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Albrecht, Doris – Learning & Memory, 2007
It is known from studies outside the brain that upon binding to its receptor, angiotensin-(1-7) elicits the release of prostanoids and nitric oxide (NO). Cyclooxygenase (COX) is a key enzyme that converts arachidonic acid to prostaglandins. Since there are no data available so far on the role of COX-2 in the amygdala, in a first step we…
Descriptors: Stimulation, Brain, Animals, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Stein, Christien Kimpel; LaBarba, Richard C. – Developmental Psychology, 1977
Descriptors: Animal Behavior, Maturation, Research, Stimulation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Masaki, Takahisa; Nakajima, Sadahiko – Learning and Motivation, 2004
In two experiments, the evidence showed that 20 min of forced swimming by rats caused aversion to a taste solution consumed before swimming. When one of two taste solutions (sodium saccharin or sodium chloride, counterbalanced across rats) was paired with swimming and the other was not, the rats' intakes of these two solutions showed less…
Descriptors: Animals, Animal Behavior, Conditioning, Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Owings, Donald H. – European Journal of Developmental Science, 2007
Gilbert Gottlieb's data and epigenetic approach support the conclusion that organisms are functionally-whole agents at each phase of development rather than simply incompletely developed adults prior to sexual maturity and deteriorated adults in old age. This implies that organisms construct distinct ontogenetic niches at each phase of…
Descriptors: Evolution, Developmental Stages, Adolescent Development, Age Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Moore, Jay; Friedlen, Karen E. – Psychological Record, 2007
Pigeons were trained in three experiments with a two-key, concurrent-chains choice procedure. The initial links were equal variable-interval schedules, and the terminal links were random-time schedules with equal average interreinforcement intervals. Across the three experiments, the pigeons either stayed in a terminal link until a reinforcer was…
Descriptors: Intervals, Reinforcement, Evaluation Methods, Hyperactivity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ward, Ryan D.; Bailey, Ericka M.; Odum, Amy L. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
This experiment assessed the effects of "d"-Amphetamine and ethanol on reinforced variable and repetitive key-peck sequences in pigeons. Pigeons responded on two keys under a multiple schedule of Repeat and Vary components. In the Repeat component, completion of a target sequence of right, right, left, left resulted in food. In the Vary component,…
Descriptors: Animals, Control Groups, Drug Use, Reinforcement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Lejeune, Helga; Richelle, Marc; Wearden, J. H. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2006
The article discusses two important influences of B. F. Skinner, and later workers in the behavior-analytic tradition, on the study of animal timing. The first influence is methodological, and is traced from the invention of schedules imposing temporal constraints or periodicities on animals in "The Behavior of Organisms," through the rate…
Descriptors: Animals, Animal Behavior, Behavioral Science Research, Scheduling
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Sapolsky, Robert M. – Social Forces, 2006
Philosophers often consider what it is that makes individuals human. For biologists considering the same, the answer is often framed in the context of what are the key differences between humans and other animals. One vestige of human uniqueness still often cited by anthropologists is culture. However, this notion has been challenged in recent…
Descriptors: Animal Behavior, Aggression, Animals, Primatology
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  21  |  22  |  23  |  24  |  25  |  26  |  27  |  28  |  29  |  ...  |  86