Publication Date
In 2025 | 9 |
Since 2024 | 101 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 489 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1336 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 3240 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
DiPerna, Paul | 13 |
Brainerd, C. J. | 10 |
Pascalis, Olivier | 9 |
Yonelinas, Andrew P. | 9 |
Moscovitch, Morris | 8 |
Tomasello, Michael | 8 |
Crossley, Scott A. | 7 |
Merriman, William E. | 7 |
Oakes, Lisa M. | 7 |
Reyna, V. F. | 7 |
Wixted, John T. | 7 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Teachers | 50 |
Researchers | 16 |
Practitioners | 7 |
Counselors | 6 |
Media Staff | 6 |
Students | 5 |
Policymakers | 4 |
Parents | 3 |
Administrators | 2 |
Community | 2 |
Support Staff | 1 |
More ▼ |
Location
Australia | 101 |
Turkey | 90 |
Canada | 72 |
United Kingdom | 70 |
China | 62 |
United States | 54 |
California | 48 |
United Kingdom (England) | 47 |
Germany | 40 |
Iran | 39 |
Netherlands | 37 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Song, Hyun-joo; Baillargeon, Renee; Fisher, Cynthia – Cognition, 2005
The present research investigated whether 13.5-month-old infants would attribute to an actor a disposition to perform a recurring action, and would then use this information to predict which of two new objects--one that could be used to perform the action and one that could not--the actor would grasp next. During familiarization, the infants…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Cognitive Ability, Familiarity, Behavioral Science Research
McArthur, Duncan; Adamson, Lauren B.; Deckner, Deborah F. – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly: Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2005
The interactions of 24-, 30-, and 36-month-old children and their mothers reading two initially unfamiliar books were observed three times over a 2-week period. Coding characterized both the content and the role of their utterances as they discussed the stories. Utterance content depended on child age but changed little with increasing story…
Descriptors: Oral Language, Preschool Children, Mothers, Parent Child Relationship

Demorest, Steven M.; Schultz, Sara J. M. – Journal of Research in Music Education, 2004
In two separate studies, the authors examined fifth graders' preference for authentic and arranged versions of world music recordings, the relationship of those preference ratings to familiarity, and teachers' ability to predict student preferences. In the first study, intact classes of fifth-grade students were randomly assigned to an authentic…
Descriptors: Grade 5, Textbooks, Familiarity, Singing
Carlson, Bonnie E.; Worden, Alissa Pollitz – Journal of Interpersonal Violence, 2005
This study reports analyses and findings from a public opinion survey designed to explore beliefs about domestic violence (DV) -- what it is, when it is against the law, and how prevalent it is. The project interviewed 1,200 residents from six New York communities. The analyses reveal substantial first hand and second hand experience with DV and…
Descriptors: Socioeconomic Background, Public Opinion, Family Violence, Aggression

Sims, Wendy L. – Journal of Research in Music Education, 2005
Preschool children's listening time responses to free versus directed listening activities were compared. For the free condition, children were instructed just to "listen as long as you would like." The directed condition was a written task designed to focus attention on specific aspects of the music and give children something concrete to do…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Preschool Children, Comparative Analysis, Attention Control
Bukach, Cindy M.; Bub, Daniel N.; Masson, Michael E. J.; Lindsay, D. Stephen – Cognitive Psychology, 2004
Studies of patients with category-specific agnosia (CSA) have given rise to multiple theories of object recognition, most of which assume the existence of a stable, abstract semantic memory system. We applied an episodic view of memory to questions raised by CSA in a series of studies examining normal observers' recall of newly learned attributes…
Descriptors: Patients, Recall (Psychology), Identification, Recognition (Psychology)
Smith, J. David; Redford, Joshua S.; Washburn, David A.; Taglialatela, Lauren A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2005
Screeners at airport security checkpoints perform an important categorization task in which they search for threat items in complex x-ray images. But little is known about how the processes of categorization stand up to visual complexity. The authors filled this research gap with screening tasks in which participants searched for members of target…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Classification, Screening Tests, Security Personnel
Abbot-Smith, Kirsten; Lieven, Elena; Tomasello, Michael – Developmental Science, 2004
Childers and Tomasello (2001) found that training 2 1/2-year-olds on the English transitive construction greatly improves their performance on a post-test in which they must use novel verbs in that construction. In the current study, we replicated Childers and Tomasello's finding, but using a much lower frequency of transitive verbs and models in…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, Familiarity, Syntax
Williams, Dana – Teaching Tolerance, 2003
This article reports on a project that challenges students to socialize with students from a variety of groups. In the observance of "Mix It Up at Lunch Day," students sit somewhere new, with someone new, in the cafeterias. More than 3,000 schools accepted the challenge as a way to bridge social boundaries in their schools.
Descriptors: Dining Facilities, Socialization, Familiarity, Interpersonal Communication
Benenson, Gary; Piggott, Felice – Journal of Industrial Teacher Education, 2002
The word "technology" has evolved through several different meanings during the recent history of education. In 1985, the American Industrial Arts Association changed its name to the International Technology Education Association. This change reflected dissatisfaction with traditional "shop" courses, designed to prepare…
Descriptors: Curriculum Development, Educational History, Familiarity, Technology Education
German, Diane J.; Newman, Rochelle S. – Reading Psychology, 2007
We examined how children with and without oral language (word-finding) difficulties (WFD) perform on oral reading (OR) versus silent reading recognition (SRR) tasks when reading the same words and how lexical factors influenced OR accuracy, error patterns, and nature of miscues. Primary-grade students were administered an experimental reading…
Descriptors: Silent Reading, Oral Reading, Oral Language, Familiarity
Baek, Mikyung; Damarin, Suzanne K. – Language and Intercultural Communication, 2008
Having grown up in an age of rapidly developing electronic communication technology, today's students come to higher education with high levels of comfort and familiarity with computer-mediated communication (CMC, hereafter). The students' level of comfort with CMC, coupled with CMC's promises of enabling supplemental class discussion as well as…
Descriptors: Higher Education, Computer Mediated Communication, Familiarity, Qualitative Research
Moore, David Richard – Journal of Interactive Online Learning, 2006
Instructional strategies for successfully teaching concepts are found throughout the instructional design literature. These strategies primarily consist of presenting learners with definitions, examples, and non-examples. While examples are important presentation instruments, theorist suggests that examples should not be re-used in the assessment…
Descriptors: Instructional Design, Concept Formation, Test Items, Student Evaluation
Yerys, Benjamin E.; Munakata, Yuko – Child Development, 2006
Children often perseverate, repeating prior behaviors when inappropriate. This work tested the roles of verbal labels and stimulus novelty in such perseveration. Three-year-old children sorted cards by one rule and were then instructed to switch to a second rule. In a basic condition, cards had familiar shapes and colors and both rules were stated…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Persistence, Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes
Senechal, Monique; Basque, Michelle T.; Leclaire, Tina – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2006
The goal of the current research was to assess whether children can make strategic use of morphological relations among words to spell. French-speaking children in Grade 4 spelled three word types: (a) phonological words that had regular phoneme-grapheme correspondences, (b) morphological words that had silent consonant endings for which a…
Descriptors: Spelling, Grade 4, Phonemes, Learning Strategies