Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 4 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 17 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 83 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1141 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Practitioners | 19 |
Researchers | 12 |
Teachers | 12 |
Administrators | 1 |
Counselors | 1 |
Location
Germany | 38 |
United Kingdom | 27 |
California | 24 |
Canada | 24 |
Israel | 22 |
New York | 20 |
United Kingdom (England) | 19 |
Australia | 18 |
Netherlands | 17 |
North Carolina | 13 |
Pennsylvania | 12 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
No Child Left Behind Act 2001 | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Noles, Nicholaus S.; Gelman, Susan A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The goal of this study was to evaluate the claim that young children display preferences for auditory stimuli over visual stimuli. This study was motivated by concerns that the visual stimuli employed in prior studies were considerably more complex and less distinctive than the competing auditory stimuli, resulting in an illusory preference for…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cues, Auditory Stimuli, Preschool Children
Collins, Sarah J.; Graham, Susan A.; Chambers, Craig G. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We investigated how preschoolers use their understanding of the actions available to a speaker to resolve referential ambiguity. In this study, 58 3- and 4-year-olds were presented with arrays of eight objects in a toy house and were instructed to retrieve various objects from the display. The trials varied in terms of whether the speaker's hands…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Language Processing, Preschool Children, Experiments
Locey, Matthew L.; Rachlin, Howard – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
Humans often make seemingly irrational choices in situations of conflict between a particular smaller-sooner reinforcer and a more abstract, temporally extended, but larger reinforcer. In two experiments, the extent to which the availability of commitment responses--self-imposed restrictions on future choices--might improve self-control in such…
Descriptors: Cooperation, Control Groups, Institutionalized Persons, Positive Reinforcement
Georgiou, George K.; Papadopoulos, Timothy C.; Fella, Argyro; Parrila, Rauno – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We examined how rapid automatized naming (RAN) components--articulation time and pause time--predict word and text reading fluency in a consistent orthography (Greek). In total, 68 children were followed from Grade 2 to Grade 6 and were assessed three times on RAN (Digits and Objects), phonological awareness, orthographic processing, speed of…
Descriptors: Reading Difficulties, Reading Fluency, Phonological Awareness, Grade 6
Sauter, Megan; Uttal, David H.; Alman, Amanda Schaal; Goldin-Meadow, Susan; Levine, Susan C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This article examines two issues: the role of gesture in the communication of spatial information and the relation between communication and mental representation. Children (8-10 years) and adults walked through a space to learn the locations of six hidden toy animals and then explained the space to another person. In Study 1, older children and…
Descriptors: Animals, Nonverbal Communication, Spatial Ability, Children
Munzer, Stefan; Zimmer, Hubert D.; Baus, Jorg – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
Current GPS-based mobile navigation assistance systems support wayfinding, but they do not support learning about the spatial configuration of an environment. The present study examined effects of visual presentation modes for navigation assistance on wayfinding accuracy, route learning, and configural learning. Participants (high-school students)…
Descriptors: Computer Assisted Instruction, Maps, Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability
Stroud, Michael J.; Menneer, Tamaryn; Cave, Kyle R.; Donnelly, Nick – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Eye movements were monitored to examine search efficiency and infer how color is mentally represented to guide search for multiple targets. Observers located a single color target very efficiently by fixating colors similar to the target. However, simultaneous search for 2 colors produced a dual-target cost. In addition, as the similarity between…
Descriptors: Evidence, Eye Movements, Search Strategies, Experiments
Lyle, Keith B.; Hanaver-Torrez, Shelley D.; Hacklander, Ryan P.; Edlin, James M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Research has shown that consistently right-handed individuals have poorer memory than do inconsistently right- or left-handed individuals under baseline conditions but more reliably exhibit enhanced memory retrieval after making a series of saccadic eye movements. From this it could be that consistent versus inconsistent handedness, regardless of…
Descriptors: Handedness, Eye Movements, Figurative Language, Individual Differences
White, K. Geoffrey – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
In many theories, forgetting is closely linked to the passage of time. In the present experiments, recall in a short-term memory task was less accurate when the retention interval included a difficult arithmetic addition task, compared with an easy task. In a novel condition, the interfering task was switched from hard to easy partway through the…
Descriptors: Intervals, Short Term Memory, Retention (Psychology), Time
Vlach, Haley A.; Ankowski, Amber A.; Sandhofer, Catherine M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Several bodies of research have found different results with regard to presentation timing, categorization, and generalization. Both presenting instances at the "same time" (simultaneous) and presenting instances "apart in time" (spacing) have been shown to facilitate generalization. In this study, we resolved these results by examining…
Descriptors: Nouns, Generalization, Experiments, Experimental Psychology
Jang, Yoonhee; Mickes, Laura; Wixted, John T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
The slope of the z-transformed receiver-operating characteristic (zROC) in recognition memory experiments is usually less than 1, which has long been interpreted to mean that the variance of the target distribution is greater than the variance of the lure distribution. The greater variance of the target distribution could arise because the…
Descriptors: Research Design, Prediction, Recognition (Psychology), Memory
Chen, Fu-Chen; Stoffregen, Thomas A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
Mariners actively adjust their body orientation in response to ship motion. On a ship at sea, we evaluated relations between standing postural activity and the performance of a precision aiming task. Standing participants (experienced mariners) maintained the beam from a handheld laser on a target. Targets were large or small, thereby varying the…
Descriptors: Workstations, Cognitive Processes, Task Analysis, Priming
Flavell, Charlotte R.; Lee, Jonathan L. C. – Learning & Memory, 2012
The basolateral amygdala (BLA) and the dorsal hippocampus (dHPC) are both structures with key roles in contextual fear conditioning. During fear conditioning, it is postulated that contextual representations of the environment are formed in the hippocampus, which are then associated with foot shock in the amygdala. However, it is not known to what…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Fear, Neurological Impairments, Context Effect
Purser, Harry R. M.; Farran, Emily K.; Courbois, Yannick; Lemahieu, Axelle; Mellier, Daniel; Sockeel, Pascal; Blades, Mark – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The aim of this study was to investigate route-learning ability in 67 children aged 5 to 11 years and to relate route-learning performance to the components of Baddeley's model of working memory. Children carried out tasks that included measures of verbal and visuospatial short-term memory and executive control and also measures of verbal and…
Descriptors: Virtual Classrooms, Short Term Memory, Long Term Memory, Children
Muller, Ulrich; Liebermann-Finestone, Dana P.; Carpendale, Jeremy I. M.; Hammond, Stuart I.; Bibok, Maximilian B. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This longitudinal study examined the concurrent and predictive relations between executive function (EF) and theory of mind (ToM) in 82 preschoolers who were assessed when they were 2, 3, and 4 years old. The results showed that the concurrent relation between EF and ToM, after controlling for age, verbal ability, and sex, was significant at 3 and…
Descriptors: Theory of Mind, Verbal Ability, Cognitive Development, Longitudinal Studies