NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards1
Showing 241 to 255 of 1,167 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Melis, Alicia P.; Altrichter, Kristin; Tomasello, Michael – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
Recent studies have shown that in situations where resources have been acquired collaboratively, children at around 3 years of age share mostly equally. We investigated 3-year-olds' sharing behavior with a collaborating partner and a free-riding partner who explicitly expressed her preference not to collaborate. Children shared more equally with…
Descriptors: Resource Allocation, Sharing Behavior, Young Children, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rescorla, Leslie; Turner, Hannah L. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2015
Purpose: This study reports age 5 morphology and syntax skills in late talkers identified at age 2 (n = 34) and typically developing comparison children (n = 20). Results: The late talkers manifested significant morphological delays at ages 3 and 4 relative to comparison peers. Based on the 14 morphemes analyzed at age 5, the only significant…
Descriptors: Morphology (Languages), Syntax, Language Acquisition, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rissman, Lilia; Goldin-Meadow, Susan – Language Learning and Development, 2017
Across a diverse range of languages, children proceed through similar stages in their production of causal language: their initial verbs lack internal causal structure, followed by a period during which they produce causative overgeneralizations, indicating knowledge of a productive causative rule. We asked in this study whether a child not…
Descriptors: Verbs, Language Acquisition, Linguistic Input, Child Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brandt, Silke; Nitschke, Sanjo; Kidd, Evan – Language Learning and Development, 2017
Structural priming is a useful laboratory-based technique for investigating how children respond to temporary changes in the distribution of structures in their input. In the current study we investigated whether increasing the number of object relative clauses (RCs) in German-speaking children's input changes their processing preferences for…
Descriptors: Priming, German, Phrase Structure, Linguistic Input
Ostler, Teresa – ZERO TO THREE, 2014
Personal names are more than just a sound or word. From the earliest stages of development, names are closely connected to a child's attachment figures and sense of identity. Like words of magic, young children first use names to beckon the parent to them. Experiences with others provide the necessary backdrop for young children to infuse names…
Descriptors: Naming, Identification (Psychology), Child Development, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kim, Sunae; Harris, Paul L. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2014
Children are able to distinguish between regular events that can occur in everyday reality and magical events that are ordinarily impossible. How do children respond to a person who brings about magical as compared with ordinary outcomes? In two studies, we tested children's acceptance of informants' claims when the informants had produced either…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Fantasy, Trust (Psychology), Comparative Analysis
Administration for Children & Families, 2018
The National American Indian Alaska Native Head Start Collaboration Office (NAIANHSCO), created under Public Law 110-134 ("Improving Head Start for School Readiness Act of 2007"), is directed "to facilitate collaboration among Head Start agencies (including Early Head Start agencies) and entities that carry out activities designed…
Descriptors: American Indians, Alaska Natives, Early Intervention, Agency Cooperation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hyman, Susan L.; Stewart, Patricia A.; Foley, Jennifer; Cain, Usa; Peck, Robin; Morris, Danielle D.; Wang, Hongyue; Smith, Tristram – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2016
To obtain information on the safety and efficacy of the gluten-free/casein-free (GFCF) diet, we placed 14 children with autism, age 3-5 years, on the diet for 4-6 weeks and then conducted a double-blind, placebo-controlled challenge study for 12 weeks while continuing the diet, with a 12-week follow-up. Dietary challenges were delivered via weekly…
Descriptors: Dietetics, Sample Size, Statistical Analysis, Statistical Significance
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wang, Ye; Hartman, Maria; Aziz, Nurul Akmar Abdul; Arora, Sonia; Shi, Lingyun; Tunison, Ellie – American Annals of the Deaf, 2017
The authors systematically reviewed peer-reviewed studies done with LENA (Language ENvironment Analysis) technology, guided by three research questions: (a) What types of studies have been conducted, and with which populations, since the launch of LENA technology?; (b) What challenges related to use of LENA technology were identified?; and (c)…
Descriptors: Literature Reviews, Technology Integration, Barriers, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Campaña, Laura V.; Ouimet, Donald A. – Journal of Visual Impairment & Blindness, 2015
Since its creation in the early 1980s, Light Box, a product developed by the American Printing House for the Blind (APH) that is designed for working on functional vision tasks with children who have visual impairments or multiple disabilities, has been an effective tool to help teach children with visual impairments to locate and track items…
Descriptors: Blindness, Visual Impairments, Assistive Technology, Handheld Devices
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kamio, Yoko; Haraguchi, Hideyuki; Stickley, Andrew; Ogino, Kazuo; Ishitobi, Makoto; Takahashi, Hidetoshi – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2015
To determine the best discriminative items for identifying young children with autism spectrum disorders (ASD), we conducted a secondary analysis using longitudinal cohort data that included the Japanese version of the 23-item modified checklist for autism in toddlers (M-CHAT-JV). M-CHAT-JV data at 18 months of age and diagnostic information…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Autism, Developmental Disabilities, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Morita, Emi – Discourse Processes: A multidisciplinary journal, 2015
This study investigates a particular behavior in talk-in-interaction that appears to be, at least in its most explicit form, relatively unique to children, that is, the behavior whereby one participant explicitly instructs another participant to say a specific phrase, after which the first participant then supplies a prefashioned response. Rather…
Descriptors: Interaction Process Analysis, Toddlers, Young Children, Speech Communication
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Carson, Valerie; Clark, Dawne; Ogden, Nancy; Harber, Vicki; Kuzik, Nicholas – Early Childhood Education Journal, 2015
In December, 2013, revised Alberta child care accreditation standards were released by the Alberta Government in Canada that included a new standard for physical activity and sedentary behavior in accredited child care settings. The main purpose of this study was to examine the effectiveness of the new accreditation standard in increasing physical…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Accreditation (Institutions), Standards, Physical Activities
Black, Lindsey I.; Vahratian, Anjel; Hoffman, Howard J. – Centers for Disease Control and Prevention, 2015
Increasing the proportion of children with voice, swallowing, speech, or language disorders who receive intervention services is a Healthy People 2020 goal (1). Timely receipt of intervention services is shown to be effective for treatment of communication disorders (2-5). Using data from the 2012 National Health Interview Survey (NHIS), this…
Descriptors: Communication Disorders, Voice Disorders, Intervention, Health
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bechtel, Sabrina; Jeschonek, Susanna; Pauen, Sabina – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
This study investigated cognitive processes underlying tool use and knowledge transfer in 24-month-olds (N = 123). Following a demonstration, participants chose a tool to reach a reward in a training transfer paradigm. Differing from previous research, various aspects considered to be relevant for children's performance were integrated within the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Conflict, Feedback (Response), Transfer of Training
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  13  |  14  |  15  |  16  |  17  |  18  |  19  |  20  |  21  |  ...  |  78