NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 11 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yuchen Tian; Gorana T. González; Tara M. Mandalaywala – Developmental Science, 2024
Although actual experiences of upward social mobility are historically low, many adolescents and adults express a "belief" in social mobility (e.g., that social status can change). Although a belief in upward mobility (e.g., that status can improve) can be helpful for economically disadvantaged adolescents and adults, a belief in upward…
Descriptors: Childrens Attitudes, Beliefs, Social Mobility, Young Children
Angeline S. Lillard – Grantee Submission, 2022
Scientists have long employed puppets in research with young children; this essay explores the validity of this practice. After considering what puppets are, their main types and history, I note the different ways puppets have been employed in research. One of these uses raises the issue of whether and when children apply their theory of mind to…
Descriptors: Young Children, Puppetry, Childrens Attitudes, Play
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Allison P. Mugno; Lindsay C. Malloy – Applied Developmental Science, 2024
115, 6-9-year-olds (M age = 7.47 years) participated in a scripted event during which the child's mother or a stranger broke a forbidden puppet and requested secrecy. Then, children were either (1) primed for the goal of honesty (prime), (2) asked to promise to tell the truth (oath), or (3) given no instructions (control) before responding to…
Descriptors: Interviews, Self Disclosure (Individuals), Young Children, Parent Child Relationship
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gena Nelson; Hannah Carter; Peter Boedeker; Mackenna Vander Tuin – Learning Environments Research, 2024
The purpose of this systematic review was to identify how the home learning environment (HLE) was measured in group design, early math intervention studies conducted in the home. Specifically, we evaluated the physical (e.g. frequency of activities) and affective (e.g. parents' beliefs, children's attitudes) aspects of the HLE. We included…
Descriptors: Parents, Young Children, Early Intervention, Mathematics Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hassinger-Das, Brenna; Dore, Rebecca A. – E-Learning and Digital Media, 2023
This study describes how 3- to 8-year-olds' justifications for their reality status judgements about people on YouTube relates to their age and previous YouTube watching experience. Using a within-subjects design, children were asked about their beliefs regarding the reality status of a purported YouTube video and provided justifications for their…
Descriptors: Social Media, Video Technology, Mass Media Use, Childrens Attitudes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Marta Bialecka; Arkadiusz Gut; Malgorzata Stepien-Nycz; Krystian Macheta; Jakub Janczura – Infant and Child Development, 2024
Previous research on children's knowledge about the mind has primarily focused on their comprehension of false beliefs, leaving the conceptualization of thoughts and thinking less explored. To address this gap, we developed a new assessment tool, the interview about the mind (IaM), to assess children's understanding of the mind. Two studies…
Descriptors: Childrens Attitudes, Theory of Mind, Cognitive Development, Beliefs
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Leshin, Rachel A.; Leslie, Sarah-Jane; Rhodes, Marjorie – Child Development, 2021
A problematic way to think about social categories is to essentialize them--to treat particular differences between people as marking fundamentally distinct social kinds. From where do these beliefs arise? Language that expresses generic claims about categories elicits some aspects of essentialism, but the scope of these effects remains unclear.…
Descriptors: Language Attitudes, Beliefs, Childrens Attitudes, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Haber, Amanda S.; Kumar, Sona C.; Corriveau, Kathleen H. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2022
Eighty-six 4- and 5-year-old children were assigned to one of four conditions, three experimental conditions, in which children read a story about a famous scientist, and one baseline condition. In the "Achievement" condition: the scientist was described as receiving awards and recognition through their lifetime, with no discussion of…
Descriptors: Young Children, Persistence, Story Reading, Books
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schleihauf, Hanna; Herrmann, Esther; Fischer, Julia; Engelmann, Jan M. – Child Development, 2022
We investigate how the ability to respond appropriately to reasons provided in discourse develops in young children. In Study 1 (N = 58, Germany, 26 girls), 4- and 5-, but not 3-year-old children, differentiated good from bad reasons. In Study 2 (N = 131, Germany, 64 girls), 4- and 5-year-old children considered both the strength of evidence for…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Young Children, Beliefs, Thinking Skills
Dore, Rebecca A.; Woolley, Jacqueline D.; Hixon, John G. – Grantee Submission, 2018
Children learn about the world through others' testimony, and much of this knowledge likely comes from parents. Furthermore, parents may sometimes want children to share their beliefs about topics on which there is no universal consensus. In discussing such topics, parents may use explicit belief statements (e.g., "Evolution is real") or…
Descriptors: Beliefs, Value Judgment, Young Children, Age Differences
Gutiérrez, Isabel T.; Menendez, David; Jiang, Matthew J.; Hernandez, Iseli G.; Miller, Peggy; Rosengren, Karl S. – Grantee Submission, 2020
A mixed-method approach was used to explore parent and child perspectives on death in Mexico. Parents' and children's death-related experiences and understanding of death were examined. While all children in this sample displayed a biological understanding of death, older children were less likely to endorse that all living things die. Children…
Descriptors: Death, Mexicans, Parent Attitudes, Childrens Attitudes