Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 18 |
Descriptor
Error Patterns | 20 |
Experimental Psychology | 20 |
Visual Stimuli | 20 |
Reaction Time | 9 |
Cognitive Processes | 8 |
Foreign Countries | 8 |
Statistical Analysis | 8 |
Memory | 7 |
College Students | 5 |
Comparative Analysis | 5 |
Cues | 5 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 12 |
Journal of Experimental… | 5 |
Cognitive Psychology | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Psicologica: International… | 1 |
Author
Abrahamse, Elger L. | 1 |
Ackroyd, Katie | 1 |
Allen, Roy | 1 |
Becker, Suzanna | 1 |
Braem, Senne | 1 |
Brass, Marcel | 1 |
Bugg, Julie M. | 1 |
Castel, Caroline | 1 |
Danjon, Juliette | 1 |
De Houwer, Jan | 1 |
De Wit, Bianca | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 20 |
Reports - Research | 19 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 11 |
Postsecondary Education | 9 |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom (England) | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
Belgium | 1 |
California | 1 |
Canada | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
Illinois | 1 |
Missouri | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
Switzerland | 1 |
United Kingdom (Scotland) | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 2 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Kinoshita, Sachiko; De Wit, Bianca; Norris, Dennis – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
In 2 variants of the color-word Stroop task, we compared 5 types of color-neutral distractors--real words (e.g., "HAT"), pseudowords (e.g., "HIX"), consonant strings (e.g., "HDK"), symbol strings (e.g., #$%), and a row of Xs (e.g., "XXX")--as well as incongruent color words (e.g., "GREEN" displayed…
Descriptors: Color, Interference (Learning), Reaction Time, Visual Stimuli
Lew, Timothy F.; Pashler, Harold E.; Vul, Edward – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
What happens to memories as we forget? They might gradually lose fidelity, lose their associations (and thus be retrieved in response to the incorrect cues), or be completely lost. Typical long-term memory studies assess memory as a binary outcome (correct/incorrect), and cannot distinguish these different kinds of forgetting. Here we assess…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Long Term Memory, Learning, Visual Stimuli
Braem, Senne; Liefooghe, Baptist; De Houwer, Jan; Brass, Marcel; Abrahamse, Elger L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Unlike other animals, humans have the unique ability to share and use verbal instructions to prepare for upcoming tasks. Recent research showed that instructions are sufficient for the automatic, reflex-like activation of responses. However, systematic studies into the limits of these automatic effects of task instructions remain relatively…
Descriptors: Responses, Context Effect, Visual Stimuli, Performance
Street, Whitney N.; Wang, Ranxiao Frances – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
The perspective-taking task is one of the most common paradigms used to study the nature of spatial memory, and better performance for certain orientations is generally interpreted as evidence of spatial representations using these reference directions. However, performance advantages can also result from the relative ease in certain…
Descriptors: Perspective Taking, Experimental Psychology, Spatial Ability, Memory
Meier, Matt E.; Kane, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Three experiments examined the relation between working memory capacity (WMC) and 2 different forms of cognitive conflict: stimulus-stimulus (S-S) and stimulus-response (S-R) interference. Our goal was to test whether WMC's relation to conflict-task performance is mediated by stimulus-identification processes (captured by S-S conflict),…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Short Term Memory, Executive Function, Cognitive Processes
Identifying Strategies in Arithmetic with the Operand Recognition Paradigm: A Matter of Switch Cost?
Thevenot, Catherine; Castel, Caroline; Danjon, Juliette; Fayol, Michel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Determining adults' and children's strategies in mental arithmetic constitutes a central issue in the domain of numerical cognition. However, despite the considerable amount of research on this topic, the conclusions in the literature are not always coherent. Therefore, there is a need to carry on the investigation, and this is the reason why we…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Arithmetic, Cognitive Processes, Recognition (Psychology)
Gul, Amara; Humphreys, Glyn W. – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2015
Congruency effects were examined using a manual response version of the Stroop task in which the relationship between the colour word and its hue on incongruent trials was either kept constant or varied randomly across different pairings within the stimulus set. Congruency effects were increased in the condition where the incongruent hue-word…
Descriptors: Experiments, Psychological Testing, Perceptual Development, Perception Tests
Scheil, Juliane; Kleinsorge, Thomas – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
In task switching, a common result supporting the notion of inhibitory processes as a determinant of switch costs is the occurrence of "n"-2 repetition costs. Evidence suggests that this effect is not affected by preparation. However, the role of preparation on preceding trials has been neglected so far. In this study, evidence for an…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Inhibition, Repetition, Cues
Longman, Cai S.; Lavric, Aureliu; Monsell, Stephen – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Switching tasks prolongs response times, an effect reduced but not eliminated by active preparation. To explore the role of attentional selection of the relevant stimulus attribute in these task-switch costs, we measured eye fixations in participants cued to identify either a face or a letter displayed on its forehead. With only 200 ms between cue…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Eye Movements, Attention, Reaction Time
Exogenous Social Identity Cues Differentially Affect the Dynamic Tracking of Individual Target Faces
Allen, Roy; Gabbert, Fiona – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
We report on an experiment to investigate the top-down effect of exogenous social identity cues on a multiple-identity tracking task, a paradigm well suited to investigate the processes of binding identity to spatial locations. Here we simulated an eyewitness event in which dynamic targets, all to be tracked with equal effort, were identified from…
Descriptors: Cues, Human Body, Attention, Bias
Kunar, Melina A.; Watson, Derrick G. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
In visual search tasks participants search for a target among distractors in strictly controlled displays. We show that visual search principles observed in these tasks do not necessarily apply in more ecologically valid search conditions, using dynamic and complex displays. A multi-element asynchronous dynamic (MAD) visual search was developed in…
Descriptors: Visual Perception, Visual Stimuli, Experimental Psychology, Undergraduate Students
Han, Xue; Becker, Suzanna – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
We investigated how humans encode large-scale spatial environments using a virtual taxi game. We hypothesized that if 2 connected neighborhoods are explored jointly, people will form a single integrated spatial representation of the town. However, if the neighborhoods are first learned separately and later observed to be connected, people will…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability, Simulated Environment, Video Games
Bugg, Julie M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
The conflict monitoring account posits that globally high levels of conflict trigger engagement of top-down control; however, recent findings point to the mercurial nature of top-down control in high conflict contexts. The current study examined the potential moderating effect of associative learning on conflict-triggered top-down control…
Descriptors: Conflict, Experimental Psychology, Associative Learning, Hypothesis Testing
Murayama, Kou; Sakaki, Michiko; Yan, Veronica X.; Smith, Garry M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
In order to examine metacognitive accuracy (i.e., the relationship between metacognitive judgment and memory performance), researchers often rely on by-participant analysis, where metacognitive accuracy (e.g., resolution, as measured by the gamma coefficient or signal detection measures) is computed for each participant and the computed values are…
Descriptors: Metacognition, Memory, Accuracy, Statistical Analysis
Mackenzie, Ian G.; Leuthold, Hartmut – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Oriet and Jolicoeur (2003) proposed that an endogenous task-set reconfiguration process acts as a hard bottleneck during which even early perceptual processing is impossible. We examined this assumption using a psychophysiological approach. Participants were required to switch between magnitude and parity judgment tasks within a predictable task…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Physiology, Intervals, Visual Perception
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2