NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 16 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ducreux, Edwige; Puentes-Neuman, Guadalupe – Early Child Development and Care, 2022
This study used an ethological approach to explore the behavioural adaptation of nineteen infants during their first six weeks in Residential Care (RC), or a Foster Family (FF) or an Infant-Mother Centre (IMC). Direct observations were conducted once a week at bath time. Observed behaviours were: sleep-wake states, visual exploration, motor…
Descriptors: Infants, Foster Care, Mothers, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kretch, Kari S.; Adolph, Karen E. – Developmental Science, 2017
How do infants plan and guide locomotion under challenging conditions? This experiment investigated the real-time process of visual and haptic exploration in 14-month-old infants as they decided whether and how to walk over challenging terrain--a series of bridges varying in width. Infants' direction of gaze was recorded with a head-mounted eye…
Descriptors: Infants, Psychomotor Skills, Visual Perception, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bidet-Ildei, Christel; Kitromilides, Elenitsa; Orliaguet, Jean-Pierre; Pavlova, Marina; Gentaz, Edouard – Developmental Psychology, 2014
In human newborns, spontaneous visual preference for biological motion is reported to occur at birth, but the factors underpinning this preference are still in debate. Using a standard visual preferential looking paradigm, 4 experiments were carried out in 3-day-old human newborns to assess the influence of translational displacement on perception…
Descriptors: Neonates, Infant Behavior, Visual Stimuli, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rohlfing, Katharina J.; Longo, Matthew R.; Bertenthal, Bennett I. – Developmental Science, 2012
Pointing, like eye gaze, is a deictic gesture that can be used to orient the attention of another person towards an object or an event. Previous research suggests that infants first begin to follow a pointing gesture between 10 and 13 months of age. We investigated whether sensitivity to pointing could be seen at younger ages employing a technique…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Attention, Infants, Motion
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kuhlmeier, Valerie A.; Troje, Nikolaus F.; Lee, Vivian – Infancy, 2010
In the present study, we examined if young infants can extract information regarding the directionality of biological motion. We report that 6-month-old infants can differentiate leftward and rightward motions from a movie depicting the sagittal view of an upright human point-light walker, walking as if on a treadmill. Inversion of the stimuli…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Visual Stimuli, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Gibson, Eleanor J.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1978
Five-month-old infants were habituated to three types of visually presented rigid motion, with duration of fixation as the dependent measure. After reaching a criterion of habituation, a fourth rigid motion (not habituated) and a deformation were presented. (Author/SS)
Descriptors: Eye Fixations, Infant Behavior, Infants, Motion
Gibson, Eleanor J. – 1977
This paper deals with research on the development of infants' ability to perceive invariant features of things and the relation of objects to both the spatial layout of the infants' environment and to themselves. Basic assumptions regarding the perception of invariance are discussed and a theoretical view of the role of motion in the development…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Infants, Motion, Perceptual Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Ball, William; Dibble, Ann – Journal of Genetic Psychology, 1980
Results of two studies were consistent with the view that 3-month-old infants perceive movement in depth during their own movement. They can move to avoid a surface just prior to contacting it. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Depth Perception, Infant Behavior, Infants, Motion
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Newman, Christopher; Atkinson, Janette; Braddick, Oliver – Developmental Psychology, 2001
Recorded reaching and looking preferences and movement kinematics among 5- to 15-month-olds divided into 3 age groups. Found that 5- to 12-month-olds preferred looking first at a large object; 8.5- to 12-month-olds showed preference for reaching to smaller (graspable) objects. Kinematic measures suggested that onset of object-oriented action…
Descriptors: Behavior Patterns, Infant Behavior, Infants, Motion
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Higgins, Carol I.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1996
Compared the postural responsiveness of seven-, eight-, and nine-month-old infants. Results indicated greater use of optic flow for postural control after a self-produced locomotor experience. Infants with endogenous (creeping) or artificial (walker) self-produced locomotor experience responded to portions of the optic flow field, whereas…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Development, Human Posture, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Dannemiller, James L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2000
Examined exogenous orienting among infants between 7 and 21 weeks of age in 2 experiments using display with multiple potential attention targets. Found that as early as 7 weeks of age, sensitivity for a small moving stimulus can be significantly influenced by the simultaneous presence of competing attention targets. Found large increases in…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Attention, Attention Control, Color
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Schmuckler, Mark A.; Tsang-Tong, Hannah Y. – Developmental Psychology, 2000
Three experiments investigated use of visual input and body movement input arising from movement through the world on spatial orientation. Experiments involved infants searching for a toy hidden in one of two containers. Findings indicated that search was best after infant movement in a lit environment prior to searching; all other conditions led…
Descriptors: Cues, Infant Behavior, Infants, Kinesthetic Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bahrick, Lorraine E.; Gogate, Lakshmi J.; Ruiz, Ivonne – Child Development, 2002
Three experiments investigated discrimination and memory of 5.5-month-olds for videotapes of women performing different activities (blowing bubbles, brushing hair, brushing teeth) or static displays after a 1-minute and a 7-week delay. Findings demonstrate the attentional salience of actions over faces in dynamic events to 5.5-month-olds. Findings…
Descriptors: Attention, Comparative Analysis, Discrimination Learning, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Eizenman, Dara R.; Bertenthal, Bennett I. – Developmental Psychology, 1998
Three experiments examined 4- and 6-month-olds' sensitivity to the unity of a partly occluded moving rod undergoing translation, rotation, or oscillation. Findings suggested that all types of common motion were not equivalent for specifying infants' perceptions of occluded objects. (Author)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Comparative Analysis, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Robin, Daniel J.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1996
Thirteen infants were presented with a moving object under two lighting conditions to investigate the role of vision in early reaching. Infants were tested twice, at 5 and 7.5 months of age. The results suggest that proprioceptive feedback and sight of the target allowed for successful reaching with limited visual information, even in relatively…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Behavior Development, Eye Hand Coordination, Infant Behavior
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2