NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards1
Showing 1,276 to 1,290 of 1,504 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wong, Andus Wing-Kuen; Chen, Hsuan-Chih – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
Five experiments were conducted to investigate how subsyllabic, syllabic, and prosodic information is processed in Cantonese monosyllabic word production. A picture-word interference task was used in which a target picture and a distractor word were presented simultaneously or sequentially. In the first 3 experiments with visually presented…
Descriptors: Syllables, Rhyme, Word Recognition, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Barner, David; Wagner, Laura; Snedeker, Jesse – Cognition, 2008
What does mass-count syntax contribute to the interpretation of noun phrases (NPs), and how much of NP meaning is contributed by lexical items alone? Many have argued that count syntax specifies reference to countable individuals (e.g., "cats") while mass syntax specifies reference to unindividuated entities (e.g., "water"). We evaluated this…
Descriptors: Verbs, Nouns, Syntax, Phrase Structure
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mirman, Daniel; Dixon, James A.; Magnuson, James S. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2008
Time course estimates from eye tracking during spoken language processing (the "visual world paradigm", or VWP) have enabled progress on debates regarding fine-grained details of activation and competition over time. There are, however, three gaps in current analyses of VWP data: consideration of time in a statistically rigorous manner,…
Descriptors: Speech, Mathematical Models, Linguistics, Oral Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ruff, Ilana; Blumstein, Sheila E.; Myers, Emily B.; Hutchison, Emmette – Brain and Language, 2008
Previous studies examining explicit semantic processing have consistently shown activation of the left inferior frontal gyrus (IFG). In contrast, implicit semantic processing tasks have shown activation in posterior areas including the superior temporal gyrus (STG) and the middle temporal gyrus (MTG) with less consistent activation in the IFG.…
Descriptors: Semantics, Vocabulary, Diagnostic Tests, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gor, Kira; Cook, Svetlana – Language Learning, 2010
There is little agreement on the mechanisms involved in second language (L2) processing of regular and irregular inflectional morphology and on the exact role of age, amount, and type of exposure to L2 resulting in differences in L2 input and use. The article contributes to the ongoing debates by reporting the results of two experiments on Russian…
Descriptors: Verbs, Morphology (Languages), Russian, Native Speakers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rah, Anne; Adone, Dany – Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 2010
This article presents new evidence from offline and online processing of garden-path sentences that are ambiguous between reduced relative clause resolution and main verb resolution. The participants of this study are intermediate and advanced German learners of English who have learned the language in a nonimmersed context. The results show that…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Sentences, Verbs, Figurative Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Meinzer, Marcus; Flaisch, Tobias; Wilser, Lotte; Eulitz, Carsten; Rockstroh, Brigitte; Conway, Tim; Gonzalez-Rothi, Leslie; Crosson, Bruce – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2009
As we age, our ability to select and to produce words changes, yet we know little about the underlying neural substrate of word-finding difficulties in old adults. This study was designed to elucidate changes in specific frontally mediated retrieval processes involved in word-finding difficulties associated with advanced age. We implemented two…
Descriptors: Phonemics, Semantics, Integrity, Young Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rost, Gwyneth C.; McMurray, Bob – Developmental Science, 2009
Infants in the early stages of word learning have difficulty learning lexical neighbors (i.e. word pairs that differ by a single phoneme), despite their ability to discriminate the same contrast in a purely auditory task. While prior work has focused on top-down explanations for this failure (e.g. task demands, lexical competition), none has…
Descriptors: Learning Problems, Phonetics, Infants, Word Recognition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stefanini, Silvia; Bello, Arianna; Caselli, Maria Cristina; Iverson, Jana M.; Volterra, Virginia – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2009
Few studies have explored the development of the gesture-speech system after the two-word stage. Aim of the present study is to examine developmental changes in speech and gesture use, in the context of a simple naming task. Fifty-one children (age range: 2;3-7;6) were divided into five age groups and requested to name pictures representing…
Descriptors: Nonverbal Communication, Children, Age Differences, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Suranyi, Zsuzsanna; Csepe, Valeria; Richardson, Ulla; Thomson, Jennifer M.; Honbolygo, Ferenc; Goswami, Usha – Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2009
It has been proposed that sensitivity to the parameters underlying speech rhythm may be important in setting up well-specified phonological representations in the mental lexicon. However, different acoustic parameters may contribute differentially to rhythm and stress in different languages. Here we contrast sensitivity to one such cue, amplitude…
Descriptors: Cues, Dyslexia, Acoustics, Hungarian
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Burton, Leslie A.; Rabin, Laura; Vardy, Susan Bernstein; Frohlich, Jonathan; Porter, Gwinne Wyatt; Dimitri, Diana; Cofer, Lucas; Labar, Douglas – Brain and Cognition, 2008
Eighteen temporal lobectomy patients (9 left, LTL; 9 right, RTL) were administered four verbal tasks, an Affective Implicit Task, a Neutral Implicit Task, an Affective Explicit Task, and a Neutral Explicit Task. For the Affective and Neutral Implicit Tasks, participants were timed while reading aloud passages with affective or neutral content,…
Descriptors: Patients, Memory, Reading Rate, Gender Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Davis, Chris; Kim, Jeesun; Forster, Kenneth I. – Cognition, 2008
This study investigated whether masked priming is mediated by existing memory representations by determining whether nonwords targets would show repetition priming. To avoid the potential confound that nonword repetition priming would be obscured by a familiarity response bias, the standard lexical decision and naming tasks were modified to make…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Familiarity, Language Processing, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ritter, Frank E.; Bibby, Peter A. – Cognitive Science, 2008
We have developed a process model that learns in multiple ways while finding faults in a simple control panel device. The model predicts human participants' learning through its own learning. The model's performance was systematically compared to human learning data, including the time course and specific sequence of learned behaviors. These…
Descriptors: Problem Solving, Artificial Intelligence, Comparative Analysis, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Trofimovich, Pavel – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2008
With a goal of investigating psycholinguistic bases of spoken word processing in a second language (L2), this study examined L2 learners' sensitivity to phonological information in spoken L2 words as a function of their L2 experience and attentional demands of a learning task. Fifty-two Chinese learners of English who differed in amount of L2…
Descriptors: Phonology, Semantics, Second Languages, Oral Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dodd, Barbara; McIntosh, Beth; Erdener, Dogu; Burnham, Denis – Clinical Linguistics & Phonetics, 2008
An example of the auditory-visual illusion in speech perception, first described by McGurk and MacDonald, is the perception of [ta] when listeners hear [pa] in synchrony with the lip movements for [ka]. One account of the illusion is that lip-read and heard speech are combined in an articulatory code since people who mispronounce words respond…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Phonology, Auditory Perception, Speech Impairments
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  82  |  83  |  84  |  85  |  86  |  87  |  88  |  89  |  90  |  ...  |  101