Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 8 |
Descriptor
Experimental Psychology | 8 |
Priming | 8 |
Task Analysis | 8 |
Foreign Countries | 5 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Experiments | 3 |
Visual Stimuli | 3 |
Attention | 2 |
Infants | 2 |
Investigations | 2 |
Models | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 3 |
Journal of Experimental… | 3 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Author
Benito, Claire T. | 1 |
Biebl, Rupert | 1 |
Chen, Fu-Chen | 1 |
Damian, Markus F. | 1 |
Defever, Emmy | 1 |
Dewhurst, Stephen A. | 1 |
Dorjee, Dusana | 1 |
Dux, Paul E. | 1 |
Gebuis, Titia | 1 |
Harris, Irina M. | 1 |
Howe, Mark L. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 8 |
Reports - Research | 8 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 4 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 6 | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 2 |
Canada | 1 |
Germany | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Knott, Lauren M.; Dewhurst, Stephen A.; Howe, Mark L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Factors that affect categorical and associative false memory illusions were investigated in 2 experiments. In Experiment 1, backward associative strength (BAS) from the list word to the critical lure and interitem connectivity were manipulated in Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) and category list types. For both recall and recognition tasks, the…
Descriptors: Priming, Language Tests, Memory, Experiments
Chen, Fu-Chen; Stoffregen, Thomas A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
Mariners actively adjust their body orientation in response to ship motion. On a ship at sea, we evaluated relations between standing postural activity and the performance of a precision aiming task. Standing participants (experienced mariners) maintained the beam from a handheld laser on a target. Targets were large or small, thereby varying the…
Descriptors: Workstations, Cognitive Processes, Task Analysis, Priming
Damian, Markus F.; Dorjee, Dusana; Stadthagen-Gonzalez, Hans – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Although it is relatively well established that access to orthographic codes in production tasks is possible via an autonomous link between meaning and spelling (e.g., Rapp, Benzing, & Caramazza, 1997), the relative contribution of phonology to orthographic access remains unclear. Two experiments demonstrated persistent repetition priming in…
Descriptors: Priming, Evidence, Spelling, Phonology
Wuhr, Peter; Biebl, Rupert – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
This study investigates the impact of working memory (WM) load on response conflicts arising from spatial (non) correspondence between irrelevant stimulus location and response location (Simon effect). The dominant view attributes the Simon effect to automatic processes of location-based response priming. The automaticity view predicts…
Descriptors: Priming, Short Term Memory, Experimental Psychology, Investigations
Harris, Irina M.; Benito, Claire T.; Dux, Paul E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
We investigated distractor processing in a dual-target rapid serial visual presentation (RSVP) task containing familiar objects, by measuring repetition priming from a priming distractor (PD) to Target 2 (T2). Priming from a visually identical PD was contrasted with priming from a PD in a different orientation from T2. We also tested the effect of…
Descriptors: Priming, Language Processing, Infants, Investigations
Leboe, Jason P.; Leboe, Launa C.; Milliken, Bruce – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
According to a transfer-appropriate processing framework, immediate priming costs arise from a match between a prime and probe event on 1 dimension and a difference between those 2 events on some other dimension (i.e., a partial match). In Experiment 1, the authors used a Stroop priming procedure to generate 6 variants of partial match, yet only 1…
Descriptors: Attention, Costs, Priming, Observation
Defever, Emmy; Sasanguie, Delphine; Gebuis, Titia; Reynvoet, Bert – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
How people process and represent magnitude has often been studied using number comparison tasks. From the results of these tasks, a comparison distance effect (CDE) is generated, showing that it is easier to discriminate two numbers that are numerically further apart (e.g., 2 and 8) compared with numerically closer numbers (e.g., 6 and 8).…
Descriptors: Models, Mathematics Tests, Kindergarten, Grade 6
Kinoshita, Sachiko; Lagoutaris, Stephanie – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
Using the same-different task, Perea, Dunabeitia, Pollatsek, and Carreiras (2009) showed that digits resembling letters ("leet digits"; e.g., 1 = "I", 4 = "A") primed pseudoword strings (e.g., "V35Z3D-VESZED"), but letters resembling digits ("leet letters") did not prime digit strings (e.g.,…
Descriptors: Priming, Short Term Memory, Experimental Psychology, Task Analysis