NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 8 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pozzan, Lucia; Gleitman, Lila R.; Trueswell, John C. – Language Learning and Development, 2016
When learning verb meanings, learners capitalize on universal linguistic correspondences between syntactic and semantic structure. For instance, upon hearing the transitive sentence "the boy is glorping the girl," 2-year-olds prefer a two-participant event (e.g., a boy making a girl spin) over two simultaneous one-participant events (a…
Descriptors: Semantics, Syntax, Ambiguity (Semantics), Linguistic Theory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Thothathiri, Malathi; Kim, Albert; Trueswell, John C.; Thompson-Schill, Sharon L. – Brain and Language, 2012
The hypothesized role of Broca's area in sentence processing ranges from domain-general executive function to domain-specific computation that is specific to certain syntactic structures. We examined this issue by manipulating syntactic structure and conflict between syntactic and semantic cues in a sentence processing task. Functional…
Descriptors: Sentences, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Language Processing, Semantics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Choi, Youngon; Trueswell, John C. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2010
An eye-tracking study explored Korean-speaking adults' and 4- and 5-year-olds' ability to recover from misinterpretations of temporarily ambiguous phrases during spoken language comprehension. Eye movement and action data indicated that children, but not adults, had difficulty in recovering from these misinterpretations despite strong…
Descriptors: Language Patterns, Child Language, Syntax, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
January, David; Trueswell, John C.; Thompson-Schill, Sharon L. – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2009
For over a century, a link between left prefrontal cortex and language processing has been accepted, yet the precise characterization of this link remains elusive. Recent advances in both the study of sentence processing and the neuroscientific study of frontal lobe function suggest an intriguing possibility: The demands to resolve competition…
Descriptors: Sentences, Figurative Language, Conflict, Language Processing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Novick, Jared M.; Thompson-Schill, Sharon L.; Trueswell, John C. – Cognition, 2008
Prior eye-tracking studies of spoken sentence comprehension have found that the presence of two potential referents, e.g., two frogs, can guide listeners toward a Modifier interpretation of "Put the frog on the napkin..." despite strong lexical biases associated with "Put" that support a Goal interpretation of the temporary ambiguity (Tanenhaus,…
Descriptors: Comprehension, Sentences, Reaction Time, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Trueswell, John C.; Tanenhaus, Michael – Language and Cognitive Processes, 1991
Three experiments investigating the use of temporal information in a reduced relative clause to interpret verb tense of the main clause found that subjects rapidly assessed temporal information to resolve tense ambiguity, demonstrating an incremental approach to comprehension that uses previous discourse to continuously update comprehension and…
Descriptors: English, Language Processing, Syntax, Tenses (Grammar)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Snedeker, Jesse; Trueswell, John C. – Cognitive Psychology, 2004
Two striking contrasts currently exist in the sentence processing literature. First, whereas adult readers rely heavily on lexical information in the generation of syntactic alternatives, adult listeners in world-situated eye-gaze studies appear to allow referential evidence to override strong countervailing lexical biases (Tanenhaus,…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Sentences, Adults, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kaiser, Elsi; Trueswell, John C. – Cognition, 2004
On-line comprehension studies of flexible word-order languages find that noncanonical ("scrambled") structures induce more difficulty than canonical structures [e.g., Hyona & Hujanen, "Q. J. Exp. Psychol." 50A (1997) 841-858], with this difference being attributed to the structural complexity/infrequency of these forms. However, by presenting…
Descriptors: Syntax, Discourse Modes, Finno Ugric Languages, Language Processing