Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 21 |
Descriptor
Experimental Psychology | 22 |
Foreign Countries | 22 |
Spatial Ability | 22 |
Cognitive Processes | 13 |
Visual Stimuli | 11 |
College Students | 9 |
Cues | 9 |
Memory | 6 |
Short Term Memory | 5 |
Undergraduate Students | 5 |
Statistical Analysis | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 8 |
Journal of Experimental… | 8 |
Journal of Experimental… | 2 |
Infant and Child Development | 1 |
Journal of Experimental Child… | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Psychological Record | 1 |
Author
Mou, Weimin | 2 |
Aram, Dorit | 1 |
Bae, Gi Yeul | 1 |
Barrouillet, Pierre | 1 |
Baus, Jorg | 1 |
Becker, Suzanna | 1 |
Bentin, Shlomo | 1 |
Biebl, Rupert | 1 |
Branigan, Holly P. | 1 |
Camos, Valérie | 1 |
Catmur, Caroline | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 22 |
Reports - Research | 21 |
Reports - Evaluative | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 15 |
Postsecondary Education | 6 |
Early Childhood Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
High Schools | 1 |
Kindergarten | 1 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 3 |
Australia | 2 |
Canada | 2 |
Hong Kong | 2 |
Israel | 2 |
South Korea | 2 |
United Kingdom | 2 |
Belgium | 1 |
Canada (Edmonton) | 1 |
Canada (Toronto) | 1 |
China | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
Wide Range Achievement Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Robin, Jessica; Wynn, Jordana; Moscovitch, Morris – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Events always unfold in a spatial context, leading to the claim that it serves as a scaffold for encoding and retrieving episodic memories. The ubiquitous co-occurrence of spatial context with events may induce participants to generate a spatial context when hearing scenarios of events in which it is absent. Spatial context should also serve as an…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Memory, Recall (Psychology), Cues
Zhang, Hui; Mou, Weimin; McNamara, Timothy P.; Wang, Lin – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Four experiments investigated the manner in which people use spatial reference directions to organize spatial memories of 2 conceptually nested layouts. Participants learned directions of 8 remote cities centered to Beijing or Edmonton, where the experiments occurred, using a map or using direct pointing. The map and the environment were aligned,…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Memory, Maps, Geographic Location
Travis, Susan L.; Mattingley, Jason B.; Dux, Paul E. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
The human visual system receives more information than can be consciously processed. To overcome this capacity limit, we employ attentional mechanisms to prioritize task-relevant (target) information over less relevant (distractor) information. Regularities in the environment can facilitate the allocation of attention, as demonstrated by the…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability, Cues, Cognitive Processes
Vergauwe, Evie; Camos, Valérie; Barrouillet, Pierre – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Working memory is typically defined as a system devoted to the simultaneous maintenance and processing of information. However, the interplay between these 2 functions is still a matter of debate in the literature, with views ranging from complete independence to complete dependence. The time-based resource-sharing model assumes that a central…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Short Term Memory, Recall (Psychology), Attention
Sewell, David K.; Smith, Philip L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
The attention literature distinguishes two general mechanisms by which attention can benefit performance: gain (or resource) models and orienting (or switching) models. In gain models, processing efficiency is a function of a spatial distribution of capacity or resources; in orienting models, an attentional spotlight must be aligned with the…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Attention Control, Experimental Psychology, Visual Stimuli
Mou, Weimin; Zhou, Ruojing – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Four experiments examined the roles of extended surfaces and the number of points in the boundary superiority effect in goal localization. Participants learned the locations of 4 objects in the presence of a boundary, landmarks, or both in an immersive virtual environment by reproducing the locations with feedback. Participants then localized the…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Feedback (Response), Geographic Location, Memory
Han, Xue; Becker, Suzanna – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
We investigated how humans encode large-scale spatial environments using a virtual taxi game. We hypothesized that if 2 connected neighborhoods are explored jointly, people will form a single integrated spatial representation of the town. However, if the neighborhoods are first learned separately and later observed to be connected, people will…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Spatial Ability, Simulated Environment, Video Games
Munzer, Stefan; Zimmer, Hubert D.; Baus, Jorg – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
Current GPS-based mobile navigation assistance systems support wayfinding, but they do not support learning about the spatial configuration of an environment. The present study examined effects of visual presentation modes for navigation assistance on wayfinding accuracy, route learning, and configural learning. Participants (high-school students)…
Descriptors: Computer Assisted Instruction, Maps, Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability
Chao, Hsuan-Fu – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
In a location-selection task, the repetition of a prior distractor location as the target location would slow down the response. This effect is termed the location negative priming (NP) effect. Recently, it has been demonstrated that repetition of a prior target location as the current target location would also slow down response. Because such…
Descriptors: Priming, Inhibition, Foreign Countries, Cues
Hu, Frank K.; Samuel, Arthur G.; Chan, Agnes S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: General, 2011
Inhibition of return (IOR) occurs when a target is preceded by an irrelevant stimulus (cue) at the same location: Target detection is slowed, relative to uncued locations. In the present study, we used relatively complex displays to examine the effect of repetition of nonspatial attributes. For both color and shape, attribute repetition produced a…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Inhibition, Habituation, Cues
Catmur, Caroline; Heyes, Cecilia – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Imitative compatibility, or automatic imitation, has been used as a measure of imitative performance and as a behavioral index of the functioning of the human mirror system (e.g., Brass, Bekkering, Wohlschlager, & Prinz, 2000; Heyes, Bird, Johnson, & Haggard, 2005; Kilner, Paulignan, & Blakemore, 2003). However, the use of imitative…
Descriptors: Evidence, Science Education, Imitation, Spatial Ability
Wuhr, Peter; Biebl, Rupert – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
This study investigates the impact of working memory (WM) load on response conflicts arising from spatial (non) correspondence between irrelevant stimulus location and response location (Simon effect). The dominant view attributes the Simon effect to automatic processes of location-based response priming. The automaticity view predicts…
Descriptors: Priming, Short Term Memory, Experimental Psychology, Investigations
Raw, Rachael K.; Kountouriotis, Georgios K.; Mon-Williams, Mark; Wilkie, Richard M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Old age is associated with poorer movement skill, as indexed by reduced speed and accuracy. Nevertheless, reductions in speed and accuracy can also reflect compensation as well as deficit. We used a manual tracing and a driving task to identify generalized spatial and temporal compensations and deficits associated with old age. In Experiment 1,…
Descriptors: Older Adults, Psychomotor Skills, Computer Simulation, Cognitive Processes
Leonhard, Tanja; Fernandez, Susana Ruiz; Ulrich, Rolf; Miller, Jeff – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Five psychological refractory period (PRP) experiments were conducted with an especially time-consuming first task (Experiments 1, 3, and 5: mental rotation; Experiments 2 and 4: memory scanning) and with equal emphasis on the first task and on the second (left-right tone judgment). The standard design with varying stimulus onset asynchronies…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Spatial Ability, Task Analysis, Cognitive Processes
Jones, Manon W.; Branigan, Holly P.; Parra, Mario A.; Logie, Robert H. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
The ability to learn visual-phonological associations is a unique predictor of word reading, and individuals with developmental dyslexia show impaired ability in learning these associations. In this study, we compared developmentally dyslexic and nondyslexic adults on their ability to form cross-modal associations (or "bindings") based…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Dyslexia, Predictor Variables, Associative Learning
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2