Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 27 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 36 |
Experimental Psychology | 36 |
Responses | 36 |
Visual Stimuli | 18 |
Foreign Countries | 12 |
Memory | 12 |
College Students | 10 |
Cues | 9 |
Inhibition | 9 |
Reaction Time | 8 |
Recall (Psychology) | 7 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental… | 22 |
Journal of Experimental… | 7 |
Journal of Experimental… | 3 |
Child Development | 1 |
Infancy | 1 |
Author
Henik, Avishai | 2 |
Klapp, Stuart T. | 2 |
Abrahamse, Elger L. | 1 |
Abrams, Lise | 1 |
Altmann, Erik M. | 1 |
Aly, Mariam | 1 |
Anderson, John R. | 1 |
Arbuthnott, Katherine D. | 1 |
Avnit, Amir | 1 |
Bell, Raoul | 1 |
Beltz, Brandon C. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 32 |
Reports - Research | 30 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 17 |
Postsecondary Education | 12 |
Two Year Colleges | 1 |
Audience
Location
Germany | 5 |
Israel | 2 |
Belgium | 1 |
California | 1 |
Florida | 1 |
Missouri | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
Spain | 1 |
Switzerland | 1 |
United Kingdom | 1 |
United Kingdom (England) | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 1 |
Wechsler Adult Intelligence… | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Durst, Moritz; Janczyk, Markus – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
A frequent observation in dual-task studies is the backward crosstalk effect (BCE), meaning that aspects of a secondary Task 2 influence Task 1 performance. Up to this point, 2 major types of the BCE were investigated: a BCE based on dimensional overlap between both stimuli and/or responses (the compatibility-based BCE), and a BCE based on whether…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Performance, Visual Stimuli, Color
Braem, Senne; Liefooghe, Baptist; De Houwer, Jan; Brass, Marcel; Abrahamse, Elger L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
Unlike other animals, humans have the unique ability to share and use verbal instructions to prepare for upcoming tasks. Recent research showed that instructions are sufficient for the automatic, reflex-like activation of responses. However, systematic studies into the limits of these automatic effects of task instructions remain relatively…
Descriptors: Responses, Context Effect, Visual Stimuli, Performance
Davis, Danielle K.; Abrams, Lise – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
When people read questions like "How many animals of each kind did Moses take on the ark?", many mistakenly answer "2" despite knowing that Noah sailed the ark. This "Moses illusion" occurs when names share semantic features. Two experiments examined whether shared "visual" concepts (facial features)…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Semantics, Visual Stimuli, Interference (Learning)
Weinbach, Noam; Kalanthroff, Eyal; Avnit, Amir; Henik, Avishai – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
The goal of the present study was to examine if and how arousal can modulate response inhibition. Two competing hypotheses can be drawn from previous literature. One holds that alerting cues that elevate arousal should result in an impulsive response and therefore impair response inhibition. The other suggests that alerting enhances processing of…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Arousal Patterns, Inhibition, Cues
Kliegl, Oliver; Pastötter, Bernhard; Bäuml, Karl-Heinz T. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Proactive interference (PI) refers to the finding that memory for recently studied (target) material can be impaired by the prior study of other (nontarget) material. Previous accounts of PI differed in whether they attributed PI to impaired retrieval or impaired encoding. Here, we suggest an integrated encoding-retrieval account, which assigns a…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Experimental Psychology, Memory, Interference (Learning)
Koen, Joshua D.; Aly, Mariam; Wang, Wei-Chun; Yonelinas, Andrew P. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
A prominent finding in recognition memory is that studied items are associated with more variability in memory strength than new items. Here, we test 3 competing theories for why this occurs--the "encoding variability," "attention failure", and "recollection" accounts. Distinguishing among these theories is critical…
Descriptors: Memory, Cognitive Processes, Attention, Recall (Psychology)
Jones, Manon W.; Snowling, Margaret J.; Moll, Kristina – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Reading fluency is often predicted by rapid automatized naming (RAN) speed, which as the name implies, measures the automaticity with which familiar stimuli (e.g., letters) can be retrieved and named. Readers with dyslexia are considered to have less "automatized" access to lexical information, reflected in longer RAN times compared with…
Descriptors: Reading Fluency, Dyslexia, Interference (Learning), Color
Smith, Megan A.; Roediger, Henry L., III; Karpicke, Jeffrey D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
Many experiments provide evidence that practicing retrieval benefits retention relative to conditions of no retrieval practice. Nearly all prior research has employed retrieval practice requiring overt responses, but a few experiments have shown that covert retrieval also produces retention advantages relative to control conditions. However,…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Memory, Drills (Practice), Experimental Psychology
Druey, Michel D. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
In many task-switch studies, task sequence and response sequence interact: Response repetitions produce benefits when the task repeats but produce costs when the task switches. Four different theoretical frameworks have been proposed to explain these effects: a reconfiguration-based account, association-learning models, an episodic-retrieval…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Repetition, Responses, Prediction
Scheil, Juliane; Kleinsorge, Thomas – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
In task switching, a common result supporting the notion of inhibitory processes as a determinant of switch costs is the occurrence of "n"-2 repetition costs. Evidence suggests that this effect is not affected by preparation. However, the role of preparation on preceding trials has been neglected so far. In this study, evidence for an…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Inhibition, Repetition, Cues
Bell, Raoul; Röer, Jan P.; Buchner, Axel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Rating the relevance of words for the imagined situation of being stranded in the grasslands without survival material leads to exceptionally good memory for these words. This survival processing effect has received much attention because it promises to elucidate the evolutionary foundations of memory. However, the proximate mechanisms of the…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Memory, Cognitive Processes, Adjustment (to Environment)
Forrest, Charlotte L. D.; Monsell, Stephen; McLaren, Ian P. L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Task-cuing experiments are usually intended to explore control of task set. But when small stimulus sets are used, they plausibly afford learning of the response associated with a combination of cue and stimulus, without reference to tasks. In 3 experiments we presented the typical trials of a task-cuing experiment: a cue (colored shape) followed,…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Cues, Visual Stimuli, Color
Spurgeon, Jessica; Ward, Geoff; Matthews, William J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
We examined the contribution of the phonological loop to immediate free recall (IFR) and immediate serial recall (ISR) of lists of between one and 15 words. Following Baddeley (1986, 2000, 2007, 2012), we assumed that visual words could be recoded into the phonological store when presented silently but that recoding would be prevented by…
Descriptors: Recall (Psychology), Word Lists, Visual Stimuli, Cognitive Processes
Mulligan, Neil W.; Spataro, Pietro; Picklesimer, Milton – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Study stimuli presented at the same time as unrelated targets in a detection task are better remembered than stimuli presented with distractors. This attentional boost effect (ABE) has been found with pictorial (Swallow & Jiang, 2010) and more recently verbal materials (Spataro, Mulligan, & Rossi-Arnaud, 2013). The present experiments…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Attention, Cognitive Processes, Memory
Declerck, Mathieu; Philipp, Andrea M.; Koch, Iring – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
To investigate bilingual language control, prior language switching studies presented visual objects, which had to be named in different languages, typically indicated by a visual cue. The present study examined language switching of predictable responses by introducing a novel sequence-based language switching paradigm. In 4 experiments,…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Bilingualism, Memory, Language Usage