NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 16 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mitsugi, Sanako; Fukuda, Haruka – First Language, 2022
This study examined Japanese-speaking mothers' passives in the child-directed speech from the CHILDES database. We selected five parent-child corpora and analyzed the overall distribution of the mothers' passives and further investigated the contribution of the construction and the passivized verbs to sentence meaning. The findings were as…
Descriptors: Japanese, Mothers, Language Usage, Speech Communication
Karen Barako Arndt – ProQuest LLC, 2019
Data from elicited language tasks can add to the literature on the development of the complex syntax structures of embedded complement clauses in typically developing children. In the current study, preschool-age children (n = 27) participated in two elicited language tasks focusing on three types of embedded complement clauses: infinitival…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Language Acquisition, Verbs, Phrase Structure
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cournane, Ailís – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2021
This paper revisits the longstanding observation that children produce modal verbs (e.g., must, could) with their root meanings (e.g., abilities, obligations) by age 2, typically a year or more earlier than with their epistemic meanings (e.g., inferences). Established explanations for this "Epistemic Gap" argue that epistemic language…
Descriptors: Verbs, Language Acquisition, Inferences, Syntax
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hsu, Ning; Rispoli, Matthew; Hadley, Pamela A. – Language Learning and Development, 2019
The Mandarin resultative verb compound (RVC; e.g., "tui dao" "push fall" and "pa shang" "climb ascend") encodes complex events composed of an initiating action and resulting activity or state. This study investigated when Mandarin-speaking children acquired this language-specific device. Specifically, we…
Descriptors: Verbs, Mandarin Chinese, Language Acquisition, Morphemes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Smolík, Filip – First Language, 2015
This article reports on an experiment that examined the comprehension of transitive sentences in Czech children and its relationship to case marking, word order and information structure. A total of 107 Czech children aged 2;9-4;5 were tested for comprehension of noun-verb-noun sentences in which word order and given-new status of individual nouns…
Descriptors: Word Order, Nouns, Verbs, Grammar
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Friend, Margaret; Pace, Amy – Developmental Psychology, 2011
The present article investigates spatial- and social-cognitive processes in toddlers' mapping of concepts to real-world events. In 2 studies we explore how event segmentation might lay the groundwork for extracting actions from the event stream and conceptually mapping novel verbs to these actions. In Study 1, toddlers demonstrated the ability to…
Descriptors: Cues, Verbs, Toddlers, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Maguire, Mandy J.; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Imai, Mutsumi; Haryu, Etsuko; Vanegas, Sandra; Okada, Hiroyuki; Pulverman, Rachel; Sanchez-Davis, Brenda – Cognition, 2010
The world's languages draw on a common set of event components for their verb systems. Yet, these components are differentially distributed across languages. At what age do children begin to use language-specific patterns to narrow possible verb meanings? English-, Japanese-, and Spanish-speaking adults, toddlers, and preschoolers were shown…
Descriptors: Verbs, Toddlers, Language Acquisition, Contrastive Linguistics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roseberry, Sarah; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Parish-Morris, Julia; Golinkoff, Roberta M. – Child Development, 2009
The availability of educational programming aimed at infants and toddlers is increasing, yet the effect of video on language acquisition remains unclear. Three studies of 96 children aged 30-42 months investigated their ability to learn verbs from video. Study 1 asked whether children could learn verbs from video when supported by live social…
Descriptors: Verbs, Preschool Children, Interpersonal Relationship, Educational Media
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Johnson, Bonnie W.; Morris, Sherrill R. – Child Language Teaching and Therapy, 2007
This study examined the effect of lexical aspect and phonology on regular past-tense production. Data are presented from a group of 31 children, mean age 33 months, with typical language development. A case study of a 50-month-old child with Specific Language Impairment (SLI) is also presented. Children imitated sentence pairs that included an…
Descriptors: Phonology, Language Acquisition, Language Impairments, Morphemes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Carter, Allyson; Gerken, Louann – Journal of Child Language, 2004
When English-speaking two-year-olds begin producing polysyllabic words, they often omit unstressed syllables that precede syllables with primary stress (Allen & Hawkins, 1980; Klein, 1981; Gerken, 1994a). One proposed mechanism for these omissions is that children omit syllables at a phonological level, due to prosodic constraints that act on…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Preschool Children, Sentences, Pronunciation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Hadley, Pamela A. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 1998
Grammatical development was examined for 10 children (ages 19 to 31 months) with expressive language impairments only and 10 children with both receptive- and expressive-language impairments. Group analyses did not reveal any differences between the subtypes on the Index of Productive Syntax. However, specific weakness in verb-phrase elaboration…
Descriptors: Child Development, Expressive Language, Grammar, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Anderson, Raquel T. – Applied Psycholinguistics, 1998
Monolingual Spanish-speaking children between the ages of 2 and 4 were given two structured tasks that assessed the contrastive use of "se" for coding these functions. Results suggest there is a differential order of acquisition of the clitic "se," whereby children initially contrast regular and reflexive with nonreflexive…
Descriptors: Child Language, Contrastive Linguistics, Language Acquisition, Language Usage
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Clark, Eve V.; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1995
Examines how young children describe reversals of action that restore objects to a prior, less-constrained, state. In both English and German, children first rely on the verb "open"; they then use their knowledge of particle pairs. Reentry into a prior state is underlined by uses of "back" and "wieder" and the…
Descriptors: Child Language, English, Foreign Countries, Form Classes (Languages)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Allen, Shanley E. M.; Crago, Martha B. – Journal of Child Language, 1996
Presents data from 4 Inuit children ages 2;0 to 3;6 that shows relatively early acquisition of both simple and complex forms of the passive. Within this age range, children are productively producing truncated, full, action, and experiential passive. Reasons for this precociousness, including adult input and language structure, are explored. (56…
Descriptors: Child Language, Developmental Stages, Eskimos, Hypothesis Testing
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moore, Chris; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1995
Examines the development of children's understanding of the difference between "want" and "need" in two different experiments. The first experiment required the children to respond verbally in choosing between the two concepts; the second required them to give an object to one of two characters who had made a request using…
Descriptors: Analysis of Variance, Child Language, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2