NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Education Level
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 32 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Can, Derya – Acta Educationis Generalis, 2021
Introduction: Subitizing, a quick apprehension of the numerosity of a small set of items, is consistently utilized to support early number understanding. Perceptual subitizing is the innate ability to recognize less than five items without consciously using other mental or mathematical processes. Conceptual subitizing, which requires higher-level…
Descriptors: Numbers, Perception, Preschool Children, Conservation (Concept)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Watanabe, Nobuki – International Electronic Journal of Mathematics Education, 2021
Conservation and executive function (EF) are important early childhood skills; however, knowledge about their relationship is scarce. Hence, in this study, this relationship is investigated, and a comparison is conducted between the Piagetian conservation and EF tasks to obtain the total hemoglobin (mMmm) for the left and right brain activity in…
Descriptors: Executive Function, Piagetian Theory, Developmental Stages, Conservation (Concept)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Halford, Graeme S.; Boyle, Frances M. – Child Development, 1985
Displays that by themselves always elicited chance judgment of number were shown to three- to four-year-olds and six- to seven-year-olds. The first display was transformed into the second, and so on. Results indicated that three- to four-year-olds do not understand conservation of number because judgements of successive displays were independent…
Descriptors: Children, Conservation (Concept), Developmental Stages, Mathematical Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Guthrie, Karen; Hudson, Lynne M. – Child Development, 1979
This study is a partial replication and extension of research reported by Golomb and Cornelius in 1977. Training in the present study was significantly less effective than in the earlier research. Findings suggest experimenter and testing effects on performance. (RH)
Descriptors: Conservation (Concept), Play, Preschool Children, Symbolism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Fuson, Karen C.; And Others – Child Development, 1983
In the first experiment, observations were made of children ages four-and-a-half to five-and-a-half years of age who were induced to use counting or matching in a Piagetian number conservation task. The spontaneous matching and counting behavior of a more mature but not yet conserving sample was investigated in the second experiment. (RH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Computation, Conservation (Concept), Numbers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Parisi, Marinella; Sias, M. Assunta – Human Development, 1985
Hypothesizes that children may misunderstand the task required by Piaget's test and that researchers may therefore underestimate the children's cognitive capacities. Tests the hypothesis by dividing 48 children of both sexes into two groups, those taking the standard tests and those taking a test restructured to limit ambiguity. (BE)
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Conservation (Concept), Preschool Children, Test Construction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Rosen, Aynn B.; Rozin, Paul – Developmental Psychology, 1993
Preschoolers made judgments about solutions in which substances were dissolved. Preschoolers (1) distinguished visual appearance from underlying reality; (2) recognized the conservation of taste, smell, and dangerous properties; and (3) by age five recognized that matter can be decomposed into pieces too tiny to be seen by the naked eye. (BC)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Conservation (Concept), Hazardous Materials
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Schiff, William – Child Development, 1983
Children 3.5 to 5.5 years of age who were unable to conserve length with Piaget's classical task did conserve length with parallel nonverbal tasks. Findings suggest that "preoperational" children apparently do not fail to conserve length because of centration, misleading perceptual information, or immature cognitive operations regarding…
Descriptors: Conservation (Concept), Logical Thinking, Nonverbal Ability, Nonverbal Tests
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Silverman, Irwin W.; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1979
The "magic" paradigm was devised to assess conservation of number in young children. Subjects were 32 three- to four-year-old children. (MP)
Descriptors: Academic Ability, Concept Formation, Conservation (Concept), Number Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Parsonson, Barry S.; Naughton, Kathleen A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1988
Results of two experiments with five-year-olds indicated that: (1) conservation can be quickly taught with lasting results; (2) training on a limited range of exemplars will produce generalized correct responses to other, untrained classes of conservation problems; and (3) children's explanations of their judgments change as a result of exposure…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Conservation (Concept), Generalization, Preschool Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Goldhaber, Jeanne – Young Children, 1992
Play dough is the perfect medium for creating, observing, and thinking about change. Whether the use of play dough is seen as play or a demonstration of physics, through its use children learn about transformations of consistency, color, and identity; and teachers observe how young children learn. (LB)
Descriptors: Class Activities, Color, Conservation (Concept), Early Childhood Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Foorman, Barbara R. – Perceptual and Motor Skills, 1981
Piaget and Inhelder's Water-level Task, the Preschool Embedded Figures Test, and a mental processing measure called Mr. Cucui were administered to 42 four-year-olds. The significant correlation between Mr. Cucui and the horizontal/vertical bottles was discussed from the neo-Piagetian perspective of Pascual-Leone's theory of constructive operators.…
Descriptors: Cognitive Style, Cognitive Tests, Conservation (Concept), Correlation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Field, Dorothy – Child Development, 1981
In a replication study, children 3 and 4 years old were given verbal rule training in order to probe the importance of identity, reversibility, and compensation explanations in training number and length concepts. Among the results, as before, identity was found to be the most significant factor in conservation acquisition. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Concept Formation, Conservation (Concept), Number Concepts
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Cowan, Richard – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1979
Two experiments were conducted to investigate children's performance on different number versions of identity and equivalence conservation tasks. Subjects were 88 children ranging in age from four to six years. (MP)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Concept Formation, Conservation (Concept), Early Childhood Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Moore, Chris; And Others – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1984
Two experiments tested the assertion that development of quantitative knowledge follows a U-shaped course with an early stage of conservation based on functional significance. Tested three age groups of children from three to six years. Results suggest the assertion is unwarranted and reveal that performance in younger children is best interpreted…
Descriptors: Cognitive Ability, Concept Formation, Conservation (Concept), Context Effect
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3