Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 3 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 44 |
Descriptor
Experiments | 52 |
Language Processing | 52 |
Phonology | 52 |
Speech | 19 |
Foreign Countries | 17 |
Language Research | 16 |
Word Recognition | 15 |
Oral Language | 12 |
Phonemes | 12 |
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
Language Acquisition | 10 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Levelt, Willem J. M. | 3 |
Ashby, Jane | 2 |
Cholin, Joana | 2 |
Morgan, James L. | 2 |
Roelofs, Ardi | 2 |
Snoeren, Natalie D. | 2 |
Vitevitch, Michael S. | 2 |
White, Katherine S. | 2 |
Abrams, Lise | 1 |
Agauas, Stephen J. | 1 |
Alario, F. -Xavier | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 52 |
Reports - Research | 34 |
Reports - Evaluative | 16 |
Reports - Descriptive | 2 |
Opinion Papers | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 15 |
Postsecondary Education | 11 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 5 | 1 |
Intermediate Grades | 1 |
Middle Schools | 1 |
Audience
Location
Canada | 4 |
Netherlands | 3 |
Spain | 2 |
Australia | 1 |
France | 1 |
India | 1 |
Kansas | 1 |
Michigan | 1 |
Minnesota | 1 |
New Zealand | 1 |
Pennsylvania | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Kukona, Anuenue – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2020
Two visual world experiments investigated the priming of form (e.g., phonology) during language processing. In Experiment 1, participants heard high cloze probability sentences like "In order to have a closer look, the dentist asked the man to open his . . ." while viewing visual arrays with objects like a predictable target mouth,…
Descriptors: Prediction, Priming, Phonology, Language Processing
Yip, Michael C. W. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2015
The present study examined the role of positional probability of syllables played in recognition of spoken word in continuous Cantonese speech. Because some sounds occur more frequently at the beginning position or ending position of Cantonese syllables than the others, so these kinds of probabilistic information of syllables may cue the locations…
Descriptors: Sino Tibetan Languages, Experiments, Word Recognition, Language Processing
White, Darcy; Besner, Derek – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2017
There are multiple reports, in the context of the time taken to read aloud, that the joint effects of stimulus quality and word frequency (a) interact when only words appear in the list but (b) are additive when nonwords are intermixed with words (O'Malley & Besner, 2008). This triple interaction has been explained in terms of the idea that…
Descriptors: Oral Reading, Stimuli, Word Frequency, Language Processing
Jared, Debra; Ashby, Jane; Agauas, Stephen J.; Levy, Betty Ann – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Three experiments examined the role of phonology in the activation of word meanings in Grade 5 students. In Experiment 1, homophone and spelling control errors were embedded in a story context and participants performed a proofreading task as they read for meaning. For both good and poor readers, more homophone errors went undetected than spelling…
Descriptors: Semantics, Reading, Grade 5, Experiments
O'Séaghdha, Pádraig G.; Frazer, Alexandra K. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Form preparation in word production, the benefit of exploiting a useful common sound (such as the first phoneme) of iteratively spoken small groups of words, is notoriously fastidious, exhibiting a seemingly categorical, all-or-none character and a corresponding susceptibility to "killers" of preparation. In particular, the presence of a…
Descriptors: Language Processing, Language Acquisition, Vocabulary Development, Phonology
MacKenzie, Heather K.; Graham, Susan A.; Curtin, Suzanne; Archer, Stephanie L. – Developmental Psychology, 2014
We explored 12-month-olds' flexibility in accepting phonotactically illegal or ill-formed word forms in a modified associative-learning task. Sixty-four English-learning infants were presented with a training phase that either clarified the purpose of a sound--object association task or left the task ambiguous. Infants were then habituated to sets…
Descriptors: Infants, Language Acquisition, English, Slavic Languages
Dubois, Cyril; Otzenberger, Helene; Gounot, Daniel; Sock, Rudolph; Metz-Lutz, Marie-Noelle – Neuropsychologia, 2012
In a noisy environment, visual perception of articulatory movements improves natural speech intelligibility. Parallel to phonemic processing based on auditory signal, visemic processing constitutes a counterpart based on "visemes", the distinctive visual units of speech. Aiming at investigating the neural substrates of visemic processing in a…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Visual Perception, Medicine, Experiments
Kinoshita, Sachiko; Norris, Dennis – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
A prime generated by transposing two internal letters (e.g., jugde) produces strong priming of the original word (judge). In lexical decision, this transposed-letter (TL) priming effect is generally weak or absent for nonword targets; thus, it is unclear whether the origin of this effect is lexical or prelexical. The authors describe the Bayesian…
Descriptors: Phonology, Language Processing, Experiments, Coding
Roux, Sebastien; Bonin, Patrick – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2012
The issue of how information flows within the lexical system in written naming was investigated in five experiments. In Experiment 1, participants named target pictures that were accompanied by context pictures having phonologically and orthographically related or unrelated names (e.g., a picture of a "ball" superimposed on a picture of…
Descriptors: Phonology, Cognitive Processes, Recognition (Psychology), Interference (Language)
Kolinsky, Regine; Lidji, Pascale; Peretz, Isabelle; Besson, Mireille; Morais, Jose – Cognition, 2009
The aim of this study was to determine if two dimensions of song, the phonological part of lyrics and the melodic part of tunes, are processed in an independent or integrated way. In a series of five experiments, musically untrained participants classified bi-syllabic nonwords sung on two-tone melodic intervals. Their response had to be based on…
Descriptors: Intervals, Vowels, Phonology, Music
Roelofs, Ardi; Piai, Vitoria; Schriefers, Herbert – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
E. Dhooge and R. J. Hartsuiker (2010) reported experiments showing that picture naming takes longer with low- than high-frequency distractor words, replicating M. Miozzo and A. Caramazza (2003). In addition, they showed that this distractor-frequency effect disappears when distractors are masked or preexposed. These findings were taken to refute…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Attention, Experiments, Semantics
Breen, Mara; Watson, Duane G.; Gibson, Edward – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2011
This paper evaluates two classes of hypotheses about how people prosodically segment utterances: (1) meaning-based proposals, with a focus on Watson and Gibson's (2004) proposal, according to which speakers tend to produce boundaries before and after long constituents; and (2) balancing proposals, according to which speakers tend to produce…
Descriptors: Local History, Sentences, Intervals, Verbs
Cholin, Joana; Levelt, Willem J. M. – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2009
In the current paper, we asked at what level in the speech planning process speakers retrieve stored syllables. There is evidence that syllable structure plays an essential role in the phonological encoding of words (e.g., online syllabification and phonological word formation). There is also evidence that syllables are retrieved as whole units.…
Descriptors: Phonology, Experiments, Language Processing, Speech Communication
Nation, Kate; Cocksey, Joanne – Cognition, 2009
Two experiments assessed whether 7-year-old children activate semantic information from sub-word orthography. Children made category decisions to visually-presented words, some of which contained an embedded word (e.g., "hip" in s"hip"). In Experiment 1 children were slower and less accurate to classify words if they contained an embedded word…
Descriptors: Phonology, Semantics, Semiotics, Word Recognition
Hitch, Graham J.; Flude, Brenda; Burgess, Neil – Journal of Memory and Language, 2009
Three experiments tested predictions of a neural network model of phonological short-term memory that assumes separate representations for order and item information, order being coded via a context-timing signal [Burgess, N., & Hitch, G. J. (1999). Memory for serial order: A network model of the phonological loop and its timing. "Psychological…
Descriptors: Short Term Memory, Models, Prediction, Experiments