NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Publication Date
In 20250
Since 20240
Since 2021 (last 5 years)0
Since 2016 (last 10 years)2
Since 2006 (last 20 years)10
Location
Germany1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Mullen Scales of Early…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 1 to 15 of 29 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bertels, Julie; Bayard, Clémence; Floccia, Caroline; Destrebecqz, Arnaud – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2018
Recent evidence for an evolved fear module in the brain comes from studies showing that adults, children and infants detect evolutionarily threatening stimuli such as snakes faster than non-threatening ones. A decisive argument for a threat detection system efficient early in life would come from data showing, in young infants, a functional…
Descriptors: Infants, Spatial Ability, Fear, Cues
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
D'Souza, Dean; D'Souza, Hana; Jones, Emily J. H.; Karmiloff-Smith, Annette – Developmental Science, 2020
Typically developing (TD) infants adapt to the social world in part by shifting the focus of their processing resources to the relevant aspects of a visual scene. Any impairment in visual orienting may therefore constrain learning and development in domains such as language. However, although something is known about visual orienting in infants at…
Descriptors: Infants, Toddlers, Attention, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wronski, Caroline; Daum, Moritz M. – Developmental Psychology, 2014
Movement perception facilitates spatial orienting of attention in infants (Farroni, Johnson, Brockbank, & Simion, 2000). In a series of 4 experiments, we investigated how orienting of attention in infancy is modulated by dynamic stimuli. Experiment 1 (N = 36) demonstrated that 5-month-olds as well as 7-month-olds orient to the direction of a…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Infants, Cues, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jakobsen, Krisztina Varga; Frick, Janet E.; Simpson, Elizabeth A. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2013
Although much research has examined the development of orienting to social directional cues (e.g., eye gaze), little is known about the development of orienting to nonsocial directional cues, such as arrows. Arrow cues have been used in numerous studies as a means to study attentional orienting, but the development of children's understanding of…
Descriptors: Cues, Attention, Orientation, Child Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bremner, J. Gavin; Hatton, Fran; Foster, Kirsty A.; Mason, Uschi – Developmental Science, 2011
Although there is much research on infants' ability to orient in space, little is known regarding the information they use to do so. This research uses a rotating room to evaluate the relative contribution of visual and vestibular information to location of a target following bodily rotation. Adults responded precisely on the basis of visual flow…
Descriptors: Infants, Adults, Spatial Ability, Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Schietecatte, Inge; Roeyers, Herbert; Warreyn, Petra – British Journal of Developmental Psychology, 2012
From the moment infants are born, they seem to prefer orienting to social stimuli, over objects and non-social stimuli. This preference lasts throughout adulthood and is believed to play a crucial role in social-communicative development. By following up a group of infants at the age of 6, 8, and 12 months, this study explored the role of social…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Infants, Orientation, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ching, Teresa Y. C.; O'Brien, Anna; Dillon, Harvey; Chalupper, Josef; Hartley, Lisa; Hartley, David; Raicevich, George; Hain, Jens – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2009
Purpose: This study examined the head orientation of young children in naturalistic settings and the acoustics of their everyday environments for quantifying the potential effects of directionality. Method: Twenty-seven children (11 with normal hearing, 16 with impaired hearing) between 11 and 78 months of age were video recorded in naturalistic…
Descriptors: Infants, Young Children, Human Body, Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Repacholi, Betty M.; Meltzoff, Andrew N. – Child Development, 2007
Two experiments examined whether 18-month-olds learn from emotions directed to a third party. Infants watched an adult perform actions on objects, and an Emoter expressed Anger or Neutral affect toward the adult in response to her actions. The Emoter then became neutral and infants were given access to the objects. Infants' actions were influenced…
Descriptors: Infants, Toddlers, Psychological Patterns, Affective Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bahrick, Lorraine E.; Lickliter, Robert; Flom, Ross – Infancy, 2006
According to the intersensory redundancy hypothesis (IRH), during early development, perception of nonredundantly specified properties is facilitated in unimodal stimulation as compared with bimodal stimulation. Later in development, attention becomes more flexible and infants can detect nonredundantly specified properties in both unimodal and…
Descriptors: Hypothesis Testing, Stimulation, Infants, Redundancy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bertin, Evelin; Bhatt, Ramesh S. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2006
Three-month-olds are sensitive to orientation changes of line drawings when they have a three-dimensional (3-D) interpretation and when the changes are defined by both 3-D depth and two-dimensional (2-D) picture plane cues [Bhatt, R. S., & Bertin, E. (2001). Pictorial cues and three-dimensional information processing in early infancy. Journal of…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Infants, Visual Discrimination, Visual Aids
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Rieser, John J. – Child Development, 1979
Apparently, six-month-old infants can encode a location relative to a landmark, but in many situations their visual search behavior is dominated by a learned egocentric code. (RH)
Descriptors: Egocentrism, Infants, Orientation, Spatial Ability
Clifton, Rachel Keen; Meyers, William J. – J Exp Child Psychol, 1969
Research supported by a Public Health Fellowship and by grant MR-11803-01 from the National Institute of Mental Health.
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Habit Formation, Heart Rate, Infants
Nelson, Keith; Kessen, William – 1969
This study tested the hypothesis that newborns selectively orient toward angular elements in their visual field. Subjects were 36 awake and alert infants under 6 days of age. For each newborn, the study compared visual attention to three separately presented stimulus patterns: a complete outline triangle, only the sides of this triangle, and only…
Descriptors: Attention, Infants, Orientation, Perceptual Development
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Dannemiller, James L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1998
Four experiments examined exogenous orienting in 3.5-month-olds. Found that sensitivity to a small moving bar was lower when most of the red bars were in the visual field contra-lateral to this probe. The distribution of color within the visual field biased attention, making it either more or less likely that the infant detected a moving stimulus.…
Descriptors: Attention, Infant Behavior, Infants, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Mckenzie, Beryl; Day, R. H. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1971
Descriptors: Eye Fixations, Infants, Operant Conditioning, Orientation
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2