Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 1 |
Descriptor
Modeling (Psychology) | 6 |
Observational Learning | 6 |
Play | 6 |
Affective Behavior | 2 |
Age Differences | 2 |
Children | 2 |
Technical Reports | 2 |
Achievement Gains | 1 |
Adults | 1 |
Autism | 1 |
Behavior Change | 1 |
More ▼ |
Source
Child Development | 2 |
Contemporary Educational… | 1 |
Early Child Development and… | 1 |
Journal of Autism and… | 1 |
Journal of Educational… | 1 |
Author
Zimmerman, Barry J. | 2 |
Arguel, Amaël | 1 |
Colliver, Yeshe | 1 |
Couloura, Sophia | 1 |
Fenson, Larry | 1 |
Gena, Angeliki | 1 |
Kinsler, Kimberly | 1 |
Koussa, Richard | 1 |
Kymissis, Effie | 1 |
Ramsay, Douglas S. | 1 |
Sagotsky, Gerald | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 6 |
Reports - Research | 6 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 2 |
Audience
Location
Australia | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Colliver, Yeshe; Arguel, Amaël – Early Child Development and Care, 2018
Play is traditionally considered the foundation of learning in the early years. Because play is characterized by free choice, it can be difficult for adults to ensure all learning is useful for children. The intervention described here took a novel approach to this problematic. It exposed 17 four-year-olds to different adult demonstrations to see…
Descriptors: Outcomes of Education, Role Models, Play, Intervention
Effects of Modeling Action Sequence on the Play of Twelve, Fifteen, and Nineteen-Month-Old Children.

Fenson, Larry; Ramsay, Douglas S. – Child Development, 1981
Examined the relation between the spontaneous occurrence in play of simple two-part action sequences and the frequency of these sequences and their components following modeling at 12, 15, and 19 months of age. Play following modeling was typically more advanced but only 19-month-old children generally were able to imitate complete sequences.…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Infants

Sagotsky, Gerald; And Others – Child Development, 1981
Modeling and direct instruction were used to train children to cooperate rather than compete. Children in treatment conditions showed significantly more cooperation than did control groups after immediate assessment. Seven weeks later age but not treatment differences were observed in generalization of training. (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Children, Competition, Cooperation
Gena, Angeliki; Couloura, Sophia; Kymissis, Effie – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2005
The purpose of this study was to modify the affective behavior of three preschoolers with autism in home settings and in the context of play activities, and to compare the effects of video modeling to the effects of "in-vivo" modeling in teaching these children contextually appropriate affective responses. A multiple-baseline design across…
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Behavior Modification, Preschool Children, Autism

Zimmerman, Barry J.; Kinsler, Kimberly – Journal of Educational Psychology, 1979
The effects of videotape exposure to a punished model on children's toy play were studied. Three levels of prohibition were used. Children who saw the videotape maintained their inhibitions over time and generalized them to an unfamiliar adult, while kindergartners who only received strong prohibitions did not. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Modeling (Psychology), Observational Learning, Play, Primary Education

Zimmerman, Barry J.; Koussa, Richard – Contemporary Educational Psychology, 1979
Preschool children interacted with an adult model who was either low or highly rewarding to the child. Later the model displayed either a high or low degree of positive affect as he played with a nonpreferred toy. The model's affect influenced both the children's ratings of and imitative play with the toy. (Author/RD)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Behavior Change, Learning Processes, Modeling (Psychology)