NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 91 to 105 of 225 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Anderson, Matthew J.; Burpee, Tara E.; Wall, Matthew J.; McGraw, Justin J. – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2013
The present study sought to determine whether post-training exposure to a novel or familiar object, encountered in either the location of the original fear conditioning (black compartment of a passive avoidance {PA} chamber) or in a neutral setting (open field where initial object training had occurred) would prove capable of reducing fear at…
Descriptors: Fear, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Conditioning, Coping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
De la Casa, L. G.; Mena, A.; Orgaz, A.; Fernandez, A. – Learning and Motivation, 2013
Contextual specificity of Latent Inhibition (LI) has been demonstrated using an ample range of experimental procedures. Context dependence has not been consistently obtained, however, when LI has been induced using a Conditioned Taste Aversion (CTA) procedure. This paper presents two experiments designed to analyze whether the context plays the…
Descriptors: Animals, Inhibition, Classical Conditioning, Change
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Suanda, Sumarga H.; Namy, Laura L. – Infancy, 2013
Infants' early communicative repertoires include both words and symbolic gestures. The current study examined the extent to which infants organize words and gestures in a single unified lexicon. As a window into lexical organization, eighteen-month-olds' ("N" = 32) avoidance of word-gesture overlap was examined and compared with…
Descriptors: Infants, Vocabulary, Nonverbal Communication, Organization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hochmann, Jean-Remy – Cognition, 2013
The classic gavagai problem exemplifies the difficulty to identify the referent of a novel word uttered in a foreign language. Here, we consider the reverse problem: identifying the referential part of a label. Assuming "gavagai" indicates a rabbit in a foreign language, it may very well mean ""a" rabbit" or ""that" rabbit". How can a learner know…
Descriptors: Word Frequency, Second Languages, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Identification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ong, Jia Hoong; Burnham, Denis; Escudero, Paola; Stevens, Catherine J. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2017
Purpose: Evidence suggests that extensive experience with lexical tones or musical training provides an advantage in perceiving nonnative lexical tones. This investigation concerns whether such an advantage is evident in learning nonnative lexical tones based on the distributional structure of the input. Method: Using an established protocol,…
Descriptors: Music, Acoustics, Intonation, Tone Languages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Beckmann, Jens F.; Goode, Natassia – Instructional Science: An International Journal of the Learning Sciences, 2014
Previous research has found that embedding a problem into a familiar context does not necessarily confer an advantage over a novel context in the acquisition of new knowledge about a complex, dynamic system. In fact, it has been shown that a semantically familiar context can be detrimental to knowledge acquisition. This has been described as the…
Descriptors: Context Effect, Familiarity, Semantics, Goal Orientation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mix, Kelly S.; Levine, Susan C.; Cheng, Yi-Ling; Young, Christopher J.; Hambrick, David Z.; Konstantopoulos, Spyros – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2017
In a previous study, Mix et al. (2016) reported that spatial skill and mathematics were composed of 2 highly correlated, domain-specific factors, with a few cross-domain loadings. The overall structure was consistent across grade (kindergarten, 3rd grade, 6th grade), but the cross-domain loadings varied with age. The present study sought to…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Mathematics Instruction, Kindergarten, Grade 3
Mix, Kelly S.; Levine, Susan C.; Cheng, Yi-Lang; Young, Christopher J.; Hambrick, David Z.; Konstantopoulos, Spyros – Grantee Submission, 2017
In a previous study, Mix et al. (2016) reported that spatial skill and mathematics were composed of 2 highly correlated, domain-specific factors, with a few cross-domain loadings. The overall structure was consistent across grade (kindergarten, 3rd grade, 6th grade), but the cross-domain loadings varied with age. The present study sought to…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Mathematics Instruction, Kindergarten, Grade 3
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Imura, Tomoko; Adachi, Ikuma; Hattori, Yuko; Tomonaga, Masaki – Developmental Science, 2013
The shadows cast by moving objects enable human adults and infants to infer the motion trajectories of objects. Nonhuman animals must also be able to discriminate between objects and their shadows and infer the spatial layout of objects from cast shadows. However, the evolutionary and comparative developmental origins of sensitivity to cast…
Descriptors: Animals, Motion, Visual Discrimination, Spatial Ability
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Huang, Yi Ting; Spelke, Elizabeth; Snedeker, Jesse – Language Learning and Development, 2013
Number words are generally used to refer to the exact cardinal value of a set, but cognitive scientists disagree about their meanings. Although most psychological analyses presuppose that numbers have exact semantics ("two" means exactly two), many linguistic accounts propose that numbers have lower-bounded semantics (at least two), and…
Descriptors: Numbers, Semantics, Adults, Young Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Caldwell-Harris, Catherine L.; Lancaster, Alia; Ladd, D. Robert; Dediu, Dan; Christiansen, Morten H. – Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 2015
This study examined whether musical training, ethnicity, and experience with a natural tone language influenced sensitivity to tone while listening to an artificial tone language. The language was designed with three tones, modeled after level-tone African languages. Participants listened to a 15-min random concatenation of six 3-syllable words.…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, Music Education, Ethnicity, Racial Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wagner, Laura; Dunfield, Kristen A.; Rohrbeck, Kristin L. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2014
In a series of two experiments, we examined 5-year-old children's motivations for learning new conventional actions. Children watched two teachers open a novel container; the teachers differed in the nonfunctional, conventional actions they used in the process. In Experiment 1, one teacher spoke with a native accent and the other spoke with a…
Descriptors: Cues, Social Influences, Social Development, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rowe, Meredith L.; Silverman, Rebecca D.; Mullan, Bridget E. – Contemporary Educational Psychology, 2013
Previous research suggests that presenting redundant nonverbal semantic information in the form of gestures and/or pictures may aid word learning in first and foreign languages. But do nonverbal supports help all learners equally? We address this issue by examining the role of gestures and pictures as nonverbal supports for word learning in a…
Descriptors: Second Language Learning, Translation, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mohring, Wenke; Libertus, Melissa E.; Bertin, Evelyn – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The speed of a moving object is a critical variable that factors into actions such as crossing a street and catching a ball. However, it is not clear when the ability to discriminate between different speeds develops. Here, we investigated speed discrimination in 6- and 10-month-old infants using a habituation paradigm showing infants events of a…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Visual Discrimination, Habituation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Iskandar, Sam; Baird, Anne D. – Journal of Psycholinguistic Research, 2014
Although several types of figurative language exist, neuropsychological tests of non-literal language have focused on proverbs. Metaphors in the form X is (a) Y (e.g., "The body's immunological response is a battle against disease.") place a lower demand on language skills and are more easily manipulated for novelty than proverbs.…
Descriptors: Figurative Language, Familiarity, Scoring, Classification
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  ...  |  15