NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Researchers1
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 46 to 60 of 89 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Verkuilen, Jay; Smithson, Michael – Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 2012
Doubly bounded continuous data are common in the social and behavioral sciences. Examples include judged probabilities, confidence ratings, derived proportions such as percent time on task, and bounded scale scores. Dependent variables of this kind are often difficult to analyze using normal theory models because their distributions may be quite…
Descriptors: Responses, Regression (Statistics), Statistical Analysis, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Criss, Amy H. – Cognitive Psychology, 2009
Models of recognition memory assume that memory decisions are based partially on the subjective strength of the test item. Models agree that the subjective strength of targets increases with additional time for encoding however the origin of the subjective strength of foils remains disputed. Under the fixed strength assumption the distribution of…
Descriptors: Test Items, Response Style (Tests), Recognition (Psychology), Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Johnson, Timothy R.; Bolt, Daniel M. – Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 2010
Multidimensional item response models are usually implemented to model the relationship between item responses and two or more traits of interest. We show how multidimensional multinomial logit item response models can also be used to account for individual differences in response style. This is done by specifying a factor-analytic model for…
Descriptors: Models, Response Style (Tests), Factor Structure, Individual Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Riviere, James; Falaise, Aurelie – Developmental Psychology, 2011
An intriguing error has been observed in toddlers presented with a 3-location search task involving invisible displacements of an object, namely, the C-not-B task. In 3 experiments, the authors investigated the dynamics of the attentional focus process that is suspected to be involved in this task. In Experiment 1, 2.5-year-old children were…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Disabilities, Toddlers, Toys
White, K. Geoffrey; Wixted, John T. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2010
Delayed matching to sample is typically a two-alternative forced-choice procedure with two sample stimuli. In this task the effects of varying the probability of reinforcers for correct choices and the resulting receiver operating characteristic are symmetrical. A version of the task where a sample is present on some trials and absent on others is…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Psychology, Probability, Gender Differences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Maydeu-Olivares, Alberto; Brown, Anna – Multivariate Behavioral Research, 2010
The comparative format used in ranking and paired comparisons tasks can significantly reduce the impact of uniform response biases typically associated with rating scales. Thurstone's (1927, 1931) model provides a powerful framework for modeling comparative data such as paired comparisons and rankings. Although Thurstonian models are generally…
Descriptors: Item Response Theory, Rating Scales, Models, Comparative Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Emons, Wilco H. M. – Applied Psychological Measurement, 2009
For valid decision making, it is essential to both the person being measured and the person or organization that is having the person measured that the observed scores adequately represent the underlying trait. This study deals with person-fit analysis of polytomous item scores to detect unusual patterns of sum scores on subsets of items. This…
Descriptors: Personality Theories, Personality Measures, Scores, Test Items
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kubinger, Klaus D. – Educational and Psychological Measurement, 2009
The linear logistic test model (LLTM) breaks down the item parameter of the Rasch model as a linear combination of some hypothesized elementary parameters. Although the original purpose of applying the LLTM was primarily to generate test items with specified item difficulty, there are still many other potential applications, which may be of use…
Descriptors: Models, Test Items, Psychometrics, Item Response Theory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hughes, Stephanie; Gabel, Rodney; Irani, Farzan; Schlagheck, Adam – Journal of Fluency Disorders, 2010
Semantic differential instruments are often used to assess fluent speakers' attitudes toward people who stutter (PWS). Such instruments are prone to response bias and often lack the power to explain respondents' general impressions of PWS. To address these concerns 149 fluent university students completed an open-ended questionnaire in which they…
Descriptors: Stuttering, Semantics, Negative Attitudes, Psychologists
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Dube, Chad; Rotello, Caren M.; Heit, Evan – Psychological Review, 2010
A belief bias effect in syllogistic reasoning (Evans, Barston, & Pollard, 1983) is observed when subjects accept more valid than invalid arguments and more believable than unbelievable conclusions and show greater overall accuracy in judging arguments with unbelievable conclusions. The effect is measured with a contrast of contrasts, comparing…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Item Analysis, Error of Measurement, Replication (Evaluation)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ostapczuk, Martin; Moshagen, Morten; Zhao, Zengmei; Musch, Jochen – Journal of Educational and Behavioral Statistics, 2009
Randomized response techniques (RRTs) aim to reduce social desirability bias in the assessment of sensitive attributes but differ regarding privacy protection. The less protection a design offers, the more likely respondents cheat by disobeying the instructions. In asymmetric RRT designs, respondents can play safe by giving a response that is…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Social Desirability, Attitude Measures, Privacy
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ziegler, Matthias; Buehner, Markus – Educational and Psychological Measurement, 2009
The impact of socially desirable responding or faking on noncognitive assessments remains an issue of strong debate. One of the main reasons for the controversy is the lack of a statistical method to model such response sets. This article introduces a new way to model faking based on the assumption that faking occurs due to an interaction between…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Models, Control Groups, Structural Equation Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Ferrando, Pere J.; Lorenzo-Seva, Urbano; Chico, Eliseo – Structural Equation Modeling: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2009
This article proposes procedures for simultaneously assessing and controlling acquiescence and social desirability in questionnaire items. The procedures are based on a semi-restricted factor-analytic tridimensional model, and can be used with binary, graded-response, or more continuous items. We discuss procedures for fitting the model (item…
Descriptors: Factor Analysis, Response Style (Tests), Questionnaires, Test Items
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Richler, Jennifer J.; Gauthier, Isabel; Wenger, Michael J.; Palmeri, Thomas J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
Researchers have used several composite face paradigms to assess holistic processing of faces. In the selective attention paradigm, participants decide whether one face part (e.g., top) is the same as a previously seen face part. Their judgment is affected by whether the irrelevant part of the test face is the same as or different than the…
Descriptors: Models, Attention, Identification (Psychology), Tests
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vogt, Vera; Broder, Arndt – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Recently, J. J. Starns and J. L. Hicks (2005) have argued that source dimensions are retrieved independently from memory. In their innovative experiment, manipulating the retrievability of 1 source feature did not affect memory for a 2nd feature. Following C. S. Dodson and A. P. Shimamura (2000), the authors argue that the source memory measure…
Descriptors: Response Style (Tests), Memory, Measures (Individuals), Simulation
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6