NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 13 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Caitlin R. Bowman; Dagmar Zeithamova – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2023
A major question for the study of learning and memory is how to tailor learning experiences to promote knowledge that generalizes to new situations. In two experiments, we used category learning as a representative domain to test two factors thought to influence the acquisition of conceptual knowledge: the number of training examples (set size)…
Descriptors: Classification, Learning Processes, Generalization, Recognition (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hu, Mingjia; Nosofsky, Robert M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2022
In a novel version of the classic dot-pattern prototype-distortion paradigm of category learning, Homa et al. (2019) tested a condition in which individual training instances never repeated, and observed results that they claimed severely challenged exemplar models of classification and recognition. Among the results was a dissociation in which…
Descriptors: Classification, Recognition (Psychology), Computation, Models
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bramley, Neil R.; Gerstenberg, Tobias; Mayrhofer, Ralf; Lagnado, David A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2018
A large body of research has explored how the time between two events affects judgments of causal strength between them. In this article, we extend this work in 4 experiments that explore the role of temporal information in causal structure induction with multiple variables. We distinguish two qualitatively different types of information: The…
Descriptors: Time, Causal Models, Associative Learning, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bukach, Cindy M.; Vickery, Timothy J.; Kinka, Daniel; Gauthier, Isabel – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
There is growing evidence that individuation experience is necessary for development of expert object discrimination that transfers to new exemplars. Individuation training in human studies has primarily used label association tasks where labels are learned at both the individual and more abstract (basic) level, and expertise criterion requires…
Descriptors: Expertise, Evidence, Models, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Capaldi, E. J.; Martins, Ana P. G.; Altman, Meaghan – Learning and Motivation, 2009
arrow]US associations also survived The memories of the unconditioned stimulus (US) and its absence (No US), symbolized as S[superscript R] and S[superscript N], respectively, may be retrieved on US or No US trials giving rise to four types of associations, S[superscript R][right arrow]US, S[superscript R][right arrow]No US, S[superscript N][right…
Descriptors: Classical Conditioning, Animal Behavior, Rewards, Experimental Psychology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Little, Daniel R.; Lewandowsky, Stephan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
Despite the fact that categories are often composed of correlated features, the evidence that people detect and use these correlations during intentional category learning has been overwhelmingly negative to date. Nonetheless, on other categorization tasks, such as feature prediction, people show evidence of correlational sensitivity. A…
Descriptors: Feedback (Response), Cues, Attention, Classification
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Brunel, Lionel; Labeye, Elodie; Lesourd, Mathieu; Versace, Remy – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2009
The aim of this study was to provide evidence that memory and perceptual processing are underpinned by the same mechanisms. Specifically, the authors conducted 3 experiments that emphasized the sensory aspect of memory traces. They examined their predictions with a short-term priming paradigm based on 2 distinct phases: a learning phase consisting…
Descriptors: Memory, Educational Technology, Experiments, Cognitive Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Metcalfe, Janet; Finn, Bridgid – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
Two processes are postulated to underlie delayed judgments of learning (JOLs)--cue familiarity and target retrievability. The two processes are distinguishable because the familiarity-based judgments are thought to be faster than the retrieval-based processes, because only retrieval-based JOLs should enhance the relative accuracy of the…
Descriptors: Cues, Familiarity, Prediction, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Scott, Ryan B.; Dienes, Zoltan – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2008
This article examines the role of subjective familiarity in the implicit and explicit learning of artificial grammars. Experiment 1 found that objective measures of similarity (including fragment frequency and repetition structure) predicted ratings of familiarity, that familiarity ratings predicted grammaticality judgments, and that the extremity…
Descriptors: Grammar, Familiarity, Second Language Learning, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vandorpe, Stefaan; de Houwer, Jan; Beckers, Tom – Learning and Motivation, 2007
Revisions of common associative learning models incorporate a within-compound association mechanism in order to explain retrospective cue competition effects (e.g., [Dickinson, A., & Burke, J. (1996). Within-compound associations mediate the retrospective revaluation of causality judgements. "Quarterly Journal of Experimental Psychology, 49B", pp.…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Memory, Inferences, Competition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hodgson, Eric; Waller, David – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2006
Four experiments required participants to keep track of the locations of (i.e., update) 1, 2, 3, 4, 6, 8, 10, or 15 target objects after rotating. Across all conditions, updating was unaffected by set size. Although some traditional set size effects (i.e., a linear increase of latency with memory load) were observed under some conditions, these…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Long Term Memory, Spatial Ability, Learning Processes
Hutchinson, J. Wesley; Lockhead, G. R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Learning and Memory, 1977
A review of some recent experiments suggested that general similarity between words might successfully function as a structural principle for semantic memory. A spatial model based on that assumption is proposed. The relation of this model to network and set-theoretic models of semantic memory is discussed, as is the relation of this model for…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Illustrations, Learning Processes, Memory
Edgell, Stephen E.; Castellan, N. John, Jr. – Journal of Experimental Psychology, 1973
In a nonmetric multiple-cue probability learning task involving 2 binary cue dimensions, it was found that Ss can learn to use configural or pattern information (a) when only the configural information is relevant, and in addition to the configural information, one or both of the cue dimensions are relevant. (Author/RK)
Descriptors: Analysis of Variance, College Students, Cues, Experimental Psychology