NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 14 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
von Hecker, Ulrich; Klauer, Karl Christoph; Wolf, Lukas; Fazilat-Pour, Masoud – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2016
Memory performance in linear order reasoning tasks (A > B, B > C, C > D, etc.) shows quicker, and more accurate responses to queries on wider (AD) than narrower (AB) pairs on a hypothetical linear mental model (A -- B -- C -- D). While indicative of an analogue representation, research so far did not provide positive evidence for spatial…
Descriptors: Memory, Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability, Visual Perception
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Suegami, Takashi; Laeng, Bruno – Brain and Cognition, 2013
It has been shown that the left and right cerebral hemispheres (LH and RH) respectively process qualitative or "categorical" spatial relations and metric or "coordinate" spatial relations. However, categorical spatial information could be thought as divided into two types: semantically-coded and visuospatially-coded categorical information. We…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Semantics, Stimuli, Brain Hemisphere Functions
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kershner, John R. – Journal of Learning Disabilities, 2016
Rapidly changing environments in day-to-day activities, enriched with stimuli competing for attention, require a cognitive control mechanism to select relevant stimuli, ignore irrelevant stimuli, and shift attention between alternative features of the environment. Such attentional orchestration is essential to the acquisition of reading skills. In…
Descriptors: Higher Education, College Students, Dyslexia, Disabilities
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Longo, Matthew R.; Haggard, Patrick – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
The perceived distance between touches on a single skin surface is larger on regions of high tactile sensitivity than those with lower acuity, an effect known as "Weber's illusion". This illusion suggests that tactile size perception involves a representation of the perceived size of body parts preserving characteristics of the somatosensory…
Descriptors: Self Concept, Human Body, Tactual Perception, Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cho, Dongbin; Proctor, Robert W. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2011
Choice reactions to a property of an object stimulus are often faster when the location of a graspable part of the object corresponds with the location of a keypress response than when it does not, a phenomenon called the object-based Simon effect. Experiments 1-3 examined this effect for variants of teapot stimuli that were oriented to the left…
Descriptors: Experiments, Stimuli, Reaction Time, Effect Size
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Addamo, Patricia K.; Farrow, Maree; Hoy, Kate E.; Bradshaw, John L.; Georgiou-Karistianis, Nellie – Brain and Cognition, 2009
Motor overflow refers to involuntary movement or muscle activity that may coincide with voluntary movement. This study examined factors influencing motor overflow in 17 children (8-11 years), and 17 adults (18-35 years). Participants performed a finger pressing task by exerting either 33% or 66% of their maximal force output using their dominant…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Stimulation, Children, Adults
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vierck, Esther; Kiesel, Andrea – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
Numbers are thought to be represented in space along a mental left-right oriented number line. Number magnitude has also been associated with the size of grip aperture, which might suggest a connection between number magnitude and intensity. The present experiment aimed to confirm this possibility more directly by using force as a response…
Descriptors: Number Concepts, Experiments, Stimuli, Reaction Time
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Belanger, Nathalie; Baum, Shari R.; Titone, Debra – Brain and Language, 2009
The neural bases of prosody during the production of literal and idiomatic interpretations of literally plausible idioms was investigated. Left- and right-hemisphere-damaged participants and normal controls produced literal and idiomatic versions of idioms ("He hit the books.") All groups modulated duration to distinguish the interpretations. LHD…
Descriptors: Sentences, Cues, Patients, Bilingualism
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shaul, Shelley – Reading and Writing: An Interdisciplinary Journal, 2012
This study examined the differences in processing between regular and dyslexic readers in a lexical decision task in different visual field presentations (left, right, and center). The research utilized behavioral measures that provide information on accuracy and reaction time and electro-physiological measures that permit the examination of brain…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Speech, Reaction Time, Oral Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Weed, Ethan; McGregor, William; Nielsen, Jorgen Feldbaek; Roepstorff, Andreas; Frith, Uta – Brain and Language, 2010
Why do people with right hemisphere damage (RHD) have difficulty with pragmatics and communication? One hypothesis has been that pragmatic impairment in RHD is the result of an underlying impairment in Theory of Mind (ToM): the ability to infer the mental states of others. In previous studies evaluating ToM abilities in people with RHD,…
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Cartoons, Geometric Concepts, Films
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Lopez-Moliner, Joan – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2005
Different asymmetries between expansion and contraction (radial motions) have been reported in the literature. Often these patterns have been regarded as implying different channels for each type of radial direction (outward versus inwards) operating at a higher level of visual motion processing. In two experiments (detection and discrimination…
Descriptors: Reaction Time, Motion, Experiments, Vision
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kosslyn, Stephen M. – Psychological Review, 1987
An alternative way of attempting to understand visual hemispheric specialization is presented. A theory of certain high-level processing subsystems is formulated in light of an analysis of problems that must be solved by the visual system and the constraints on the solutions to these problems. (Author/LMO)
Descriptors: Brain Hemisphere Functions, Cognitive Processes, Encoding (Psychology), Lateral Dominance
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Chokron, Sylvie; Colliot, Pascale; Atzeni, Thierry; Bartolomeo, Paolo; Ohlmann, Theophile – Brain and Cognition, 2004
Eighty blindfolded healthy female subjects participated in an active and a passive straight-ahead pointing task to study the estimation of the subjective sagittal middle in the presence or absence of an active haptic exploration. Subjects were to point straight-ahead with their left or right index finger starting from different right- or…
Descriptors: Females, Spatial Ability, Motion, Task Analysis
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
PDF on ERIC Download full text
Gawryszewski, Luiz G.; Carreiro, Luiz Renato R.; Magalhaes, Fabio V. – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2005
A non-informative cue (C) elicits an inhibition of manual reaction time (MRT) to a visual target (T). We report an experiment to examine if the spatial distribution of this inhibitory effect follows Polar or Cartesian coordinate systems. C appeared at one out of 8 isoeccentric (7[degrees]) positions, the C-T angular distances (in polar…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reaction Time, Mathematics Activities, Cues