NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Elementary and Secondary…1
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 76 to 90 of 709 results Save | Export
Goodson, Barbara; Layzer, Carolyn – National Institute for Literacy, 2009
This paper is intended for early childhood caregivers--teachers in centers and those caring for children in homes. It is intended to help caregivers learn more about how children develop the ability to "use words" to communicate their thoughts and needs and ask questions, and to "understand language they hear" in conversations and in books. Why is…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Interpersonal Communication, Oral Language, Language Skills
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kenkel, James; Yates, Robert – Written Communication, 2009
In the tradition of work by Shaughnessy (1977) and Bartholomae (1980) applying concepts from second language acquisition research to developing writing, we explore the commonalities of L1 and L2 writers on the specific level of linguistic choices needed to order information within and across sentence boundaries. We propose that many of the kinds…
Descriptors: Native Speakers, Second Language Learning, Sentences, College Students
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yu, Xia – System: An International Journal of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics, 2009
This study investigates whether two different means of learning the word sequence "despite the fact (that)" lead to differential outcomes of the acquisition of the grammatical structure imbedded in the word string which is, due to L1 transfer, especially problematic for Chinese learners of English. The participants of the study were randomly…
Descriptors: Grammar, Memorization, English (Second Language), Second Language Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Allum, Paul H.; Wheeldon, Linda R. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Four experiments investigate the scope of grammatical planning during spoken sentence production in Japanese and English. Experiment 1 shows that sentence latencies vary with length of sentence-initial subject phrase. Exploiting the head-final property of Japanese, Experiments 2 and 3 extend this result by showing that in a 2-phrase subject…
Descriptors: Oral Language, Language Processing, Grammar, Sentence Structure
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mawlawi Diab, Nuwar – System: An International Journal of Educational Technology and Applied Linguistics, 2010
Previous research on the effects of peer-editing in bringing about language development generally examined the linguistic performance of only a few students and did not focus on specific language errors nor used a control group. To counteract these limitations, this study used a pre-test/post-test comparison group quasi-experimental design to…
Descriptors: Experimental Groups, Control Groups, Quasiexperimental Design, Sentence Structure
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Onozuka, Hiromi – Language Sciences, 2007
Rappaport Hovav and Levin [Rappaport Hovav, M., Levin, B., 1998. "Building verb meanings." In: Butt, M., Geuder, W. (Eds.), "The Projection of Arguments: Lexical and Compositional Factors." CSLI Publications, Stanford, pp. 97-134] contend that result verbs disallow object deletion because of their lexical semantic properties. Their point is that…
Descriptors: Semantics, Verbs, English, Language Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Henry, Nicholas; Culmana, Hillah; VanPattena, Bill – Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 2009
The role of explicit information (EI) as an independent variable in instructed SLA is largely underresearched. Using the framework of processing instruction, however, a series of offline studies has found no effect for EI (e.g., Benati, 2004; Sanz & Morgan-Short, 2004; VanPatten & Oikkenon, 1996). Fernandez (2008) presented two online experiments…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Word Order, Sentence Structure, Predictor Variables
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Van Dyke, Julie A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
Evidence from 3 experiments reveals interference effects from structural relationships that are inconsistent with any grammatical parse of the perceived input. Processing disruption was observed when items occurring between a head and a dependent overlapped with either (or both) syntactic or semantic features of the dependent. Effects of syntactic…
Descriptors: Interference (Language), Semantics, Comprehension, Sentence Structure
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Friedmann, Na'ama – Language Acquisition: A Journal of Developmental Linguistics, 2007
SV sentences with unaccusative verbs like "The leaf fell" involve movement from object to subject position. This line of studies tested whether young children can produce this movement and whether they represent SV sentences with unaccusatives as derived by movement. In Hebrew, unaccusatives appear in both SV and VS orders. This optionality allows…
Descriptors: Semitic Languages, Sentence Structure, Speech, Grammar
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Rawson, Katherine A. – Cognitive Psychology, 2007
Eight experiments evaluated a core assumption of several theories of text processing, the shared resource assumption, which states that component text processes share limited processing resources. Short texts each contained two critical sentences that together warranted a causal inference. The syntactic structure of the second sentence was either…
Descriptors: Inferences, Word Processing, Syntax, Word Order
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Demestre, Josep; Garcia-Albea, Jose E. – Cognitive Science, 2007
Event-related brain potentials were recorded while subjects listened to sentences containing a controlled infinitival complement. Subject and object control items were used, both with 2 potential antecedents in the upper clause. Half of the sentences had a gender agreement violation between the null subject of the infinitival complement and an…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Neurolinguistics, Language Processing, Error Analysis (Language)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roland, Douglas; Dick, Frederic; Elman, Jeffrey L. – Journal of Memory and Language, 2007
Many recent models of language comprehension have stressed the role of distributional frequencies in determining the relative accessibility or ease of processing associated with a particular lexical item or sentence structure. However, there exist relatively few comprehensive analyses of structural frequencies, and little consideration has been…
Descriptors: Sentence Structure, Psycholinguistics, Grammar, Child Language
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hale, John – Cognitive Science, 2006
A word-by-word human sentence processing complexity metric is presented. This metric formalizes the intuition that comprehenders have more trouble on words contributing larger amounts of information about the syntactic structure of the sentence as a whole. The formalization is in terms of the conditional entropy of grammatical continuations, given…
Descriptors: Sentences, Sentence Structure, Grammar, Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lucas, Margery M. – Language and Speech, 1987
Study investigated the processing of ambiguous words that varied in frequency of use of their multiple interpretations. Results indicate that, whereas lexical access is an autonomous process, selection of the appropriate interpretation is a post-lexical process that is influenced by frequency information and context. (MM)
Descriptors: Ambiguity, Language Processing, Language Research, Linguistics
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Goldin, Mark G. – Hispania, 1974
Descriptors: Grammar, Language Research, Linguistics, Sentence Structure
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  ...  |  48