Publication Date
In 2025 | 6 |
Since 2024 | 46 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 173 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 426 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 850 |
Descriptor
Language Acquisition | 1384 |
Toddlers | 1384 |
Infants | 454 |
Child Language | 360 |
Vocabulary Development | 327 |
Child Development | 285 |
Foreign Countries | 276 |
Parent Child Relationship | 245 |
Young Children | 196 |
Comparative Analysis | 180 |
Longitudinal Studies | 179 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Researchers | 60 |
Practitioners | 39 |
Parents | 24 |
Teachers | 19 |
Policymakers | 4 |
Students | 4 |
Administrators | 2 |
Community | 1 |
Support Staff | 1 |
Location
Australia | 23 |
Germany | 16 |
Italy | 15 |
United Kingdom | 15 |
Canada | 14 |
Finland | 11 |
France | 11 |
Illinois | 11 |
Netherlands | 11 |
United States | 11 |
Hong Kong | 10 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Individuals with Disabilities… | 2 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Does not meet standards | 1 |

Marchman, Virginia A.; Bates, Elizabeth – Journal of Child Language, 1994
This paper outlines the degree to which age and verb vocabulary size are predictive of changes in the reported usage of English verbs that are irregular in their past tense form in a sample of more than 1,000 children. (Contains 40 references.) (JL)
Descriptors: Age, Child Language, English, Language Acquisition

Tamis-LeMonda, Catherine S.; And Others – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 1992
Compared the language and play of U.S. and Japanese toddlers and their mothers. In both cultures, variability in toddler language and play was associated with variability in maternal language and play stimulation. U.S. toddlers were more advanced in productive and receptive vocabularies, whereas Japanese toddlers were more advanced in symbolic…
Descriptors: Cross Cultural Studies, Cultural Differences, Foreign Countries, Home Visits

Paul, Rhea; Smith, Rita L. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1993
Children slow in expressive language development (SELD) at age two and matched normal toddlers were reevaluated at age four. Fifty-seven percent of SELD children showed chronic deficits in expressive syntax and morphology at reevaluation. Children with chronic language delay performed more poorly on narrative skill than their normal language…
Descriptors: Expressive Language, Followup Studies, Language Acquisition, Language Handicaps

Smetana, Judith G.; Braeges, Judith L. – Merrill-Palmer Quarterly, 1990
Examines the development of toddlers' moral and conventional judgments and effects of language development on such judgments. Rudimentary distinctions between familiar moral, social and conventional transgressions are made during the child's third year. (Author/BB)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Day Care, Early Childhood Education, Language Acquisition

Olswang, Lesley B.; Bain, Barbara A. – Topics in Language Disorders, 1991
This article describes available models for structuring language intervention with toddlers who demonstrate a primary deficit in language acquisition. It examines ways in which intervention can change behaviors through facilitation, induction, and maintenance; how to select a service delivery system; effective teaching strategies; and clinical…
Descriptors: Behavior Change, Communication Skills, Delivery Systems, Early Intervention

Stevens, Tassos; Karmiloff-Smith, Annette – Journal of Child Language, 1997
This study examines the processes underlying vocabulary acquisition, i.e., how new words are learned, in children with Williams Syndrome, a rare neurodevelopmental disorder. A Williams Syndrome group was compared to groups of normal controls in the range 3-9 years in four different experiments testing for constraints on word learning. (Author/JL)
Descriptors: Child Language, Classification, Cognitive Mapping, Comparative Analysis

McCaffrey, Helen A.; Davis, Barbara L.; MacNeilage, Peter F.; von Hapsburg, Deborah – Volta Review, 1999
A case study of a child who was stimulated with a cochlear implant at age 25 months is reported. Postimplantation, nasals decreased and other consonant types increased, particularly alveolars. The vowel space expanded, including increased production of diphthongs. Serial organization of speech postimplantation mirrored basic motor propensities in…
Descriptors: Case Studies, Cochlear Implants, Consonants, Deafness

Rescorla, Leslie – Annals of Dyslexia, 2000
Language and reading outcomes at age 13 were examined in 22 children who were late talkers as toddlers. Compared to 14 controls, children who were late talkers had significantly poorer vocabulary, grammar, reading/spelling, and verbal memory skills, although they performed in the average range on most language and academic tasks. (Contains…
Descriptors: Adolescents, Followup Studies, Grammar, Language Acquisition

Anderson, Raquel T. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 1998
Forty monolingual, Puerto Rican, Spanish-speaking children (ages 2-3) were given two tasks designed to obligate production of nominative and object pronouns in both reflexive and non-reflexive forms. In contrast to English-speaking children, these children demonstrated a pattern in which nominate-pronoun use preceded object-case use. (Author/CR)
Descriptors: Developmental Stages, Language Acquisition, Language Impairments, Language Patterns

Snyder, Lynn S.; Yoshinaga-Itano, Christine – Volta Review, 1999
This study, with 180 deaf or hard of hearing infants and toddlers grouped into four age ranges, found highly significant interrelationships among the four categories of play and the four aspects of communicative development studied. The development of autosymbolic play, age of hearing loss identification, and symbolic object distribution accounted…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Development, Deafness, Disability Identification
DiCarlo, Cynthia F.; Stricklin, Sarintha; Banajee, Meher; Reid, Dennis H. – Journal of the Association for Persons with Severe Handicaps (JASH), 2001
Evaluation of effects of manual signing on toddlers' verbal and nonverbal communicative behaviors in an inclusive preschool found that teacher signing was accompanied by increases in communicative interactions by toddlers with and without disabilities. No reductive effects on communicative verbalizations were observed for either group. (Contains…
Descriptors: Disabilities, Early Intervention, Inclusive Schools, Language Acquisition

Yoder, Paul J.; McCathren, Rebecca B.; Warren, Steven F.; Watson, Amy L. – Communication Disorders Quarterly, 2001
Receptive language, prelinguistic communication, maternal responses, vocabulary level, and expressive language were measured at six-month intervals for 58 young children with disabilities or developmental delays and their primary caretakers. Results supported the importance of caregivers responding differentially to preintentional and intentional…
Descriptors: Caregiver Speech, Developmental Delays, Disabilities, Expressive Language

Girolametto, Luigi; Weitzman, Elaine; van Lieshout, Riet; Duff, Dawna – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2000
Five subtypes of directiveness were examined in the interactions of day care teachers with toddler and preschooler groups. Results confirmed that instructional context is an important mediator of teachers' directiveness and suggest that subtypes of directiveness have different effects on child language output. (Contains references.) (Author/DB)
Descriptors: Caregiver Speech, Classroom Communication, Day Care, Expressive Language
Mintz, Toben H. – Developmental Psychology, 2005
Two hundred forty English-speaking toddlers (24- and 36-month-olds) heard novel adjectives applied to familiar objects (Experiment 1) and novel objects (Experiment 2). Children were successful in mapping adjectives to target properties only when information provided by the noun, in conjunction with participants' knowledge of the objects, provided…
Descriptors: Toddlers, Pragmatics, Nouns, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Younger, Barbara A.; Johnson, Kathy E. – Cognitive Psychology, 2004
Infants' understanding of "toy model-real exemplar" relations was assessed through preferential looking and habituation tasks. Results from the preferential looking task suggest that 18-month toddlers are just beginning to demonstrate comprehension of symbolic relations between iconic models and their real object counterparts. Performance of 10-…
Descriptors: Toys, Infants, Habituation, Toddlers