NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Laws, Policies, & Programs
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 91 to 105 of 199 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Namy, Laura L.; Waxman, Sandra R. – Child Development, 1998
Three experiments examined the relation between language acquisition and other symbolic abilities in 18- and 26-month-olds. Found that 18-month-olds spontaneously interpreted gestures, like words, as names for object categories. At 26 months, they spontaneously interpreted words as names and novel gestures as names only when given additional…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cowley, Stephen J.; Moodley, Sheshni; Fiori-Cowley, Agnese – Mind, Culture, and Activity, 2004
The article examines how infants are first permeated by culture. Building on Thibault (2000), semiogenesis is traced to the joint activity of primary intersubjectivity. Using an African example, analysis shows how--at 14 weeks--an infant already uses culturally specific indicators of "what a caregiver wants." Human predispositions and…
Descriptors: Infants, Culture, Semiotics, Mothers
Jusczyk, Peter W.; Thompson, Elizabeth – 1977
This study explored three aspects of the 2-month-old's perception of multisyllabic utterances. Questions addressed were: (1) Do infants perceive phonetic contrasts occurring either in the initial (Bada-Gada) or medial (Daba-Daga) positions of multisyllabic utterances; (2) Are infants more likely to perceive these contrasts in stressed as opposed…
Descriptors: Auditory Discrimination, Auditory Perception, Auditory Stimuli, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Murray, Lynne; Trevarthen, Colwyn – Journal of Child Language, 1986
Describes an experiment which tested the infant's sensitivity to the timing of the mother's responses by arranging a video system so that mother and baby each saw a full-face, life-size image of the other on a video screen. Results provide evidence for the infant's active role in interaction with adults. (SED)
Descriptors: Child Language, Infant Behavior, Interaction, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Lezine, Irene – International Journal of Early Childhood, 1974
This article emphasizes the importance of studying prelinguistic communication in infants. Motor development and environmental influences are seen to be important aspects of linguistic development. The possible link between sensorimotor activity and the syntactic structure of language development needs further exploration. (MS)
Descriptors: Communication (Thought Transfer), Environmental Influences, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Oller, D. Kimbrough; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1976
This research disputes the traditional position on babbling by showing that the phonetic content of babbled utterances exhibits many of the same preferences for certain kinds of phonetic elements and sequences that have been found in the production of meaningful speech by children in later stages of language development. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Language, Infant Behavior, Infants, Language Acquisition
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Greenfield, Patricia Marks – Language and Speech, 1973
Descriptors: Child Development, Child Language, Early Childhood Education, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Olswang, Lesley Barrett; Carpenter, Robert L. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1982
Some of the findings of a longitudinal study of three infants between their 11th and 22nd months to document development of linguistic expression of the agent concept indicated that first vocalizations were inconsistently associated with nonverbal agentive behaviors and later mature utterances coded agent-action-recipient events. (MC)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation, Developmental Stages
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Thomas, David G.; And Others – Child Development, 1981
Seeks to determine (1) whether 11- and 13-month-old infants directed their eye fixations to the referent of an object word said by the mother, and (2) whether there was a developmental shift in responding to object words at these two ages. Controls were set for response bias, stimulus preference, and maternal cuing. (Author/RH)
Descriptors: Comprehension, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Davis, Barbara L.; MacNeilage, Peter F. – Journal of Speech and Hearing Research, 1995
This article evaluates the "Frames, then Content" hypothesis for speech acquisition, which sees babbling as a direct result of producing syllabic "frames" by rhythmic mandibular oscillation with little of the "content" seen under mandible-independent control. Analysis of 6,659 utterances of 6 normally developing…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Child Language, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
McCune, Lorraine – Developmental Psychology, 1995
Proposes a theoretical sequence of cognitive developments as influencing representational play and language in the second year of life. Structural and temporal links between play and language indicate influence of developing mental representation, but variation in timing of developments points to influence of variables. (ET)
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Early Childhood Education, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Waxman, Sandra R. – Cognition, 1999
This study examined how novel words foster the formation of object categories for 12- to 13-month olds. Results indicated that by 12 to 13 months, infants have begun to distinguish between novel words presented as count nouns versus adjectives in fluent, infant-directed speech, and that infants' expectations for novel words accord with this…
Descriptors: Adjectives, Classification, Cognitive Development, Concept Formation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Woodward, Amanda L.; Hoyne, Karen L. – Child Development, 1999
Two studies examined whether 1-year olds' name learning during joint attention was guided by expectation that names will be in the form of spoken words. Results showed that 13-month olds, but not 20-month olds, learned a new sound/object correspondence, as evidenced by their choosing targets reliably in responses to hearing the word or sound on…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Associative Learning, Cognitive Development, Expectation
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Casasola, Marianella; Cohen, Leslie B. – Developmental Psychology, 2000
Six experiments examined infants' ability to associate nonsense words with two causal actions: pushing and pulling. Eighteen-month-olds, but not 14-month-olds, formed word-action associations. Fourteen-month-olds discriminated a change in label but not a change in action when the other was held constant. When language labels were replaced with…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Association (Psychology), Comprehension, Discrimination Learning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Carter, Anne L. – Journal of Child Language, 1975
Through discussion and illustrative events, an evolving segment of communication is described during the course of transition of one child's total communication system from the sensorimotor or gestural level at 12 months into the level of use of the adult words "more" and "mine," and associated utterances, at 24 months. (Author/RM)
Descriptors: Child Development, Child Language, Infant Behavior, Language Acquisition
Pages: 1  |  2  |  3  |  4  |  5  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  ...  |  14