Publication Date
In 2025 | 3 |
Since 2024 | 22 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 82 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 224 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 427 |
Descriptor
Expressive Language | 852 |
Language Acquisition | 852 |
Receptive Language | 365 |
Language Skills | 183 |
Vocabulary Development | 183 |
Toddlers | 175 |
Young Children | 173 |
Preschool Children | 138 |
Foreign Countries | 122 |
Language Impairments | 119 |
Child Development | 111 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Paul, Rhea | 14 |
Rescorla, Leslie | 12 |
Yoder, Paul J. | 9 |
Goldstein, Howard | 8 |
Lord, Catherine | 7 |
Warren, Steven F. | 6 |
Brady, Nancy C. | 5 |
Chapman, Robin S. | 5 |
Stokes, Stephanie F. | 5 |
Abbeduto, Leonard | 4 |
Adamson, Lauren B. | 4 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 63 |
Preschool Education | 47 |
Elementary Education | 38 |
Primary Education | 20 |
Kindergarten | 15 |
Grade 1 | 11 |
Grade 2 | 8 |
Adult Education | 5 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 4 |
High Schools | 4 |
Grade 3 | 3 |
More ▼ |
Location
United Kingdom | 14 |
Australia | 9 |
California | 6 |
Canada | 6 |
China | 6 |
Germany | 6 |
Hong Kong | 6 |
Netherlands | 6 |
Turkey | 6 |
United Kingdom (England) | 6 |
United States | 6 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Education for All Handicapped… | 1 |
Education of the Handicapped… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Meets WWC Standards with or without Reservations | 1 |
Does not meet standards | 3 |
Liegeois, Frederique; Morgan, Angela T.; Stewart, Lorna H.; Cross, J. Helen; Vogel, Adam P.; Vargha-Khadem, Faraneh – Brain and Language, 2010
Hemispherectomy (disconnection or removal of an entire cerebral hemisphere) is a rare surgical procedure used for the relief of drug-resistant epilepsy in children. After hemispherectomy, contralateral hemiplegia persists whereas gross expressive and receptive language functions can be remarkably spared. Motor speech deficits have rarely been…
Descriptors: Articulation (Speech), Children, Receptive Language, Profiles
Fattal-Valevski, Aviva; Azouri-Fattal, Iris; Greenstein, Yoram J.; Guindy, Michal; Blau, Ayala; Zelnik, Nathanel – Developmental Medicine & Child Neurology, 2009
The aim of this study was to investigate the language development of 20 children who had been exposed to thiamine (vitamin B[subscript 1]) deficiency in infancy due to feeding with soy-based formula that was accidentally deficient of thiamine. In this case-control study, 20 children (12 males, eight females; mean age 31.8 mo [SD 4.1], range 24-39…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Listening Comprehension, Physical Activities, Females
Baker, Jason K.; Messinger, Daniel S.; Lyons, Kara K.; Grantz, Caroline J. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2010
Unstructured mother-toddler interactions were examined in 18-month-old high- and low-risk children subsequently diagnosed (n = 12) or not diagnosed (n = 21) with autism spectrum disorders (ASD) at 36 months. Differences in maternal sensitivity were not found as a function of emergent ASD status. A differential-susceptibility moderation model of…
Descriptors: Behavior Problems, Autism, Child Behavior, Parent Child Relationship
Evans, Julia L.; Saffran, Jenny R.; Robe-Torres, Kathryn – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2009
Purpose: In this study, the authors examined (a) whether children with specific language impairment (SLI) can implicitly compute the probabilities of adjacent sound sequences, (b) if this ability is related to degree of exposure, (c) if it is domain specific or domain general and, (d) if it is related to vocabulary. Method: Children with SLI and…
Descriptors: Control Groups, Language Impairments, Vocabulary Development, Probability
Shneidman, Laura Ann – ProQuest LLC, 2010
Theories of language acquisition have highlighted the importance of adult speakers as active participants in children's language learning. However, in many communities children are reported to be rarely directly engaged by their caregivers. This raises the possibility that children in these communities learn language from observing 3 rd party…
Descriptors: Evidence, Maya (People), Vocabulary Development, Linguistic Input
Munasinghe, Sujeeva A.; Oliff, Carolyn; Finn, Judith; Wray, John A. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2010
To examine the effects of a digestive enzyme supplement in improving expressive language, behaviour and other symptoms in children with Autism Spectrum Disorder. Randomized, double-blind placebo-controlled trial using crossover design over 6 months for 43 children, aged 3-8 years. Outcome measurement tools included monthly Global Behaviour Rating…
Descriptors: Autism, Rating Scales, Biochemistry, Expressive Language
Tager-Flusberg, Helen; Rogers, Sally; Cooper, Judith; Landa, Rebecca; Lord, Catherine; Paul, Rhea; Rice, Mabel; Stoel-Gammon, Carol; Wetherby, Amy; Yoder, Paul – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2009
Purpose: The aims of this article are twofold: (a) to offer a set of recommended measures that can be used for evaluating the efficacy of interventions that target spoken language acquisition as part of treatment research studies or for use in applied settings and (b) to propose and define a common terminology for describing levels of spoken…
Descriptors: Pervasive Developmental Disorders, Expressive Language, Oral Language, Language Acquisition
Zamuner, Tania S. – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2009
Purpose: To examine the role of phonotactic probabilities at the onset of language development, in a new language (Dutch), while controlling for word position. Method: Using a nonword imitation task, 64 Dutch-learning children (age 2;2-2;8 [years;months]) were tested on how they imitated segments in low- and high-phonotactic probability…
Descriptors: Indo European Languages, Language Acquisition, Children, Imitation
Mashburn, Andrew J.; Justice, Laura M.; Downer, Jason T.; Pianta, Robert C. – Child Development, 2009
This study examined associations between peers' expressive language abilities and children's development of receptive and expressive language among 1,812 four-year olds enrolled in 453 classrooms in 11 states that provide large-scale public pre-kindergarten (pre-k) programs. Higher peer expressive language abilities were positively associated with…
Descriptors: Peer Influence, Child Development, Language Acquisition, Young Children
Mancilla-Martinez, Jeannette; Lesaux, Nonie K. – Child Development, 2011
This longitudinal study modeled growth rates, from ages 4.5 to 11, in English and Spanish oral language and word reading skills among 173 Spanish-speaking children from low-income households. Individual growth modeling was employed using scores from standardized measures of word reading, expressive vocabulary, and verbal short-term language…
Descriptors: Oral Language, National Norms, Reading Skills, Language Skills
Woolfe, Tyron; Herman, Rosalind; Roy, Penny; Woll, Bencie – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2010
Background: There is a dearth of assessments of sign language development in young deaf children. This study gathered age-related scores from a sample of deaf native signing children using an adapted version of the MacArthur-Bates CDI (Fenson et al., 1994). Method: Parental reports on children's receptive and expressive signing were collected…
Descriptors: Sign Language, Deafness, Vocabulary Development, Gender Bias
Bavin, Edith L.; Grayden, David B.; Scott, Kim; Stefanakis, Toni – Language and Speech, 2010
Infants' auditory processing abilities have been shown to predict subsequent language development. In addition, poor auditory processing skills have been shown for some individuals with specific language impairment. Methods used in infant studies are not appropriate for use with young children, and neither are methods typically used to test…
Descriptors: Intervals, Speech Impairments, Testing, Young Children
Finestack, Lizbeth H.; Richmond, Erica K.; Abbeduto, Leonard – Topics in Language Disorders, 2009
Fragile X syndrome (FXS) is the leading inherited cause of intellectual disability. The syndrome is caused by a single gene mutation on the X chromosome. Although individual differences are large, most individuals with FXS display weaknesses across all language and literacy domains compared with peers of the same chronological age with typical…
Descriptors: Age, Mental Retardation, Etiology, Comparative Analysis
Stolt, Suvi; Haataja, Leena; Lapinleimu, Helena; Lehtonen, Liisa – Journal of Communication Disorders, 2009
The aim of this longitudinal study was to obtain information on the early lexical development and its predictive value to language skills in very-low-birth-weight (VLBW) children. The MacArthur Communicative Development Inventory was used to collect data of the early receptive and expressive lexicon of the 32 VLBW children. This information was…
Descriptors: Body Weight, Infants, Language Acquisition, Longitudinal Studies
Richards, Janet C. – Reading Improvement, 2010
Studies indicate thoughtfully planned chants integrated with shared book reading help young children remember concepts and vocabulary they hear in literature, capture children's imagination, develop their rhyming acuity, and background knowledge, and increase their sense of story structure, understanding of story sequence, phonological awareness,…
Descriptors: Reading Aloud to Others, Phonological Awareness, Young Children, Memory