Publication Date
In 2025 | 3 |
Since 2024 | 21 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 82 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 195 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 463 |
Descriptor
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 72 |
Preschool Education | 7 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Higher Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Researchers | 83 |
Parents | 45 |
Practitioners | 21 |
Teachers | 7 |
Support Staff | 2 |
Community | 1 |
Students | 1 |
Location
Japan | 25 |
United States | 23 |
Canada | 21 |
Australia | 17 |
Sweden | 16 |
Germany | 15 |
United Kingdom (England) | 10 |
Israel | 9 |
Netherlands | 9 |
California | 8 |
Finland | 7 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Education of the Handicapped… | 1 |
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Individuals with Disabilities… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Luo, Yuyan; Baillargeon, Renee; Brueckner, Laura; Munakata, Yuko – Cognition, 2003
This study examined two alternative interpretations of violation-of-expectation findings that young infants can represent hidden objects. Findings indicated that 5-month-olds succeeded in reasoning about the interaction of a visible and a hidden object even though the 2 objects were never simultaneously visible and a 3- or 4-minute delay preceded…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Infants, Memory

Kagan, Jerome – Child Development, 1997
Notes that 4-month olds who show a low threshold to become distressed and motorically aroused to unfamiliar stimuli are more likely than others to become fearful and subdued during early childhood, while infants who show a high arousal threshold are more likely to become bold and sociable. Considers implications for psychopathology and relation…
Descriptors: Arousal Patterns, Infant Behavior, Infants, Personality

Tellinghuisen, Donald J.; Oakes, Lisa M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1997
Two experiments investigated the role of distractor characteristics and type of object-directed attention on 7- and 10-month-old infants' distraction latencies during object exploration. Found that infants took longer to turn toward distractors during focused object-directed attention than when engaged in more casual attention. They exhibited…
Descriptors: Attention Control, Infant Behavior, Infants, Responses

Sawyers, Janet K.; Rogers, Cosby S. – Young Children, 2003
Offers suggestions for facilitating infant play for the following age groups: birth to 4 months, 4 to 8 months, 8 to 12 months, and 12 to 18 months. Suggestions cover play materials and some activities. (KB)
Descriptors: Adult Child Relationship, Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants

Chambers, Kyle E.; Onishi, Kristine H.; Fisher, Cynthia – Cognition, 2003
Two experiments investigated whether novel phonotactic regularities, not present in English, could be acquired by 16.5-month-olds from brief auditory experience. Subjects listened to consonant-vowel-consonant syllables in which particular consonants were artificially restricted to either initial or final position. Findings in a subsequent…
Descriptors: Infant Behavior, Infants, Language Processing, Learning Processes

Tallandini, Maria A.; And Others – Early Child Development and Care, 1990
Studied the organization of the behaviors involved in the development of prehension by 80 infants of 1-8 months. Four types of behavior patterns that occurred at different periods were found to constitute the development of prehension. (BG)
Descriptors: Behavior Patterns, Individual Development, Infant Behavior, Infants

Dunham, Philip; And Others – Child Development, 1989
Compared effects of contingent and noncontingent adult-infant social interactions on subsequent infant-controlled habituation and choice tasks of 26 infants of 3 months. Infants who experienced a prior noncontingent social interaction tended to adopt response strategies that reduced the density of stimulation during subsequent nonsocial tasks.…
Descriptors: Adult Child Relationship, Infant Behavior, Infants, Social Behavior

Field, Tiffany; And Others – Child Development, 1988
A total of 73 three- to six-month-old infants of depressed and nondepressed mothers were videotaped in face-to-face interactions with their mothers and with nondepressed female strangers. Depressed mothers and their infants received lower ratings on all behaviors than did nondepressed dyads. (SKC)
Descriptors: Depression (Psychology), Infant Behavior, Infants, Mothers

Balaban, Marie T. – Child Development, 1995
While 18 5-month-old infants viewed photographic slides of faces posed in happy, neutral, or angry expressions, a brief acoustic noise burst was presented to elicit the blink component of human startle. It was found that blink size was augmented during the viewing of angry expressions and reduced during viewing of happy expressions. (MDM)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Emotional Response, Facial Expressions, Infant Behavior

Gardner, Judith M.; Karmel, Bernard Z. – Developmental Psychology, 1995
Looking preferences to visual temporal frequencies were examined in 77 infants at 3 ages (newborn, 1 month, and 4 months) in 3 conditions: less aroused (after feeding), more aroused-internal (before feeding), and more aroused-external (after feeding with 8 Hertz visual stimulation). Found that infants preferred faster stimuli when less aroused and…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Arousal Patterns, Infant Behavior, Infants

Picchetti, Patricia M. – Montessori Life, 1993
Describes fetal development patterns within the framework of concurrent separateness from and oneness with the mother. Touches on the infant developmental stages along the way to becoming a wholly separate being. (HTH)
Descriptors: Developmental Stages, Infant Behavior, Infants, Parent Child Relationship

Pickens, Jeffrey; Field, Tiffany – Developmental Psychology, 1993
Facial expressions were examined in 84 3-month-old infants of mothers classified as depressed, nondepressed, or low scoring on the Beck Depression Inventory. Infants of both depressed and low-scoring mothers showed significantly more sadness and anger expressions and fewer interest expressions than infants of nondepressed mothers. (Author/MDM)
Descriptors: Anger, Depression (Psychology), Facial Expressions, Infant Behavior

Baldwin, Dare A.; And Others – Child Development, 1993
Nine- to 16-month-old infants explored pairs of novel toys in 2 conditions: violated expectation, in which the first toy produced an interesting nonobvious property and the second toy did not; and interest control, in which neither toy produced the interesting property. Infants persistently attempted to reproduce the interesting property in the…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Exploratory Behavior, Infant Behavior, Infants

Zelazo, Nancy A.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1993
The effects of practice on 2 elementary neuromotor patterns, stepping and sitting, were investigated in 32 6-week-old male infants. After 7 weeks, infants who received elicitation of the stepping pattern, alone or in combination with sitting exercises, stepped more than infants who received no exercise or sitting exercise only. (MDM)
Descriptors: Child Development, Drills (Practice), Infant Behavior, Infants

Dannemiller, James L. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1998
Four experiments examined exogenous orienting in 3.5-month-olds. Found that sensitivity to a small moving bar was lower when most of the red bars were in the visual field contra-lateral to this probe. The distribution of color within the visual field biased attention, making it either more or less likely that the infant detected a moving stimulus.…
Descriptors: Attention, Infant Behavior, Infants, Models