NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 361 to 375 of 1,773 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Trauble, Birgit; Marinovic, Vesna; Pauen, Sabina – Infancy, 2010
Recent studies suggest that even infants attend to others' beliefs in order to make sense of their behavior. To warrant the assumption of early belief understanding, corresponding competences need to be demonstrated in a variety of different belief-inducing situations. The present study provides corresponding evidence, using a completely nonverbal…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Development, Infant Behavior, Competence
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Graf, Frauke; Lamm, Bettina; Goertz, Claudia; Kolling, Thorsten; Freitag, Claudia; Spangler, Sibylle; Fassbender, Ina; Teubert, Manuel; Vierhaus, Marc; Keller, Heidi; Lohaus, Arnold; Schwarzer, Gudrun; Knopf, Monika – Infant and Child Development, 2012
Three-month-old Cameroonian Nso farmer and German middle-class infants were compared regarding learning and retention in a computerized mobile task. Infants achieving a preset learning criterion during reinforcement were tested for immediate and long-term retention measured in terms of an increased response rate after reinforcement and after a…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Infants, Middle Class, Learning Processes
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Troseth, Georgene L. – Developmental Review, 2010
This paper offers an overview of research on infants' early behavior toward televised images, followed by an account of the development of "representational competence" with video. Several aspects of representation are involved in young children's understanding and use of video. From a very young age, children form mental representations of the…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Infants, Television Viewing, Behavior Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
MacPherson, Amy C.; Moore, Chris – Infancy, 2010
Infants (n = 24, mean age 13 months and n = 24, mean age 19 months) were tested on an extension of the method introduced by Tomasello and Haberl (2003) to examine the understanding of another person's interest in a novel object. Four objects were presented serially. For two objects, infants played with an experimenter. The infant played with one…
Descriptors: Infants, Child Behavior, Infant Behavior, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Braungart-Rieker, Julia M.; Hill-Soderlund, Ashley L.; Karrass, Jan – Developmental Psychology, 2010
Two goals guided this study: (a) describe changes in infant fear and anger reactivity from 4 to 16 months and (b) examine the degree to which infant temperament, attentional regulation, and maternal sensitivity predict reactivity trajectories. Participants included 143 mothers and infants (57% male) who visited the laboratory at 4, 8, 12, and 16…
Descriptors: Mothers, Infants, Infant Behavior, Personality
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Micali, Nadia; Simonoff, Emily; Stahl, Daniel; Treasure, Janet – Journal of Child Psychology and Psychiatry, 2011
Background: Maternal eating disorders (ED) have been shown to increase the risk of feeding difficulties in the offspring. Very few studies, however, have investigated whether the effect of a maternal ED on childhood feeding is a direct effect or whether it can be ascribed to other child or maternal factors. We aimed to determine the role of…
Descriptors: Early Intervention, Mothers, Eating Disorders, Child Health
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hespos, Susan J.; Saylor, Megan M.; Grossman, Stacy R. – Developmental Psychology, 2009
In a series of 3 experiments, the authors examined 6- and 8-month-old infants' capacities to detect target actions in a continuous action sequence. In Experiment 1, infants were habituated to 2 different target actions and subsequently were presented with 2 continuous action sequences that either included or did not include the familiar target…
Descriptors: Infants, Experiments, Visual Stimuli, Intention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Capuozzo, Robert M.; Sheppard, Bruce S.; Uba, Gregory – Young Children, 2010
Early childhood professionals know that good fathering has a profound impact on children. Research confirms that when fathers are involved in the lives of their children, positive outcomes can be expected; when fathers are not involved in their children's lives, more negative outcomes can be seen. Fathers can play an integral role as attachment…
Descriptors: Young Children, Parent Child Relationship, Fathers, Males
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
He, Jie; Degnan, Kathryn Amey; McDermott, Jennifer Martin; Henderson, Heather A.; Hane, Amie Ashley; Xu, Qinmei; Fox, Nathan A. – Infancy, 2010
The relations among infant anger reactivity, approach behavior, and frontal electroencephalogram (EEG) asymmetry, and their relations to inhibitory control and behavior problems in early childhood were examined within the context of a longitudinal study of temperament. Two hundred nine infants' anger expressions to arm restraint were observed at 4…
Descriptors: Behavior Problems, Infants, Inhibition, Psychological Patterns
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Zieber, Nicole; Bhatt, Ramesh S.; Hayden, Angela; Kangas, Ashley; Collins, Rebecca; Bada, Henrietta – Infancy, 2010
Like faces, bodies are significant sources of social information. However, research suggests that infants do not develop body representation (i.e., knowledge about typical human bodies) until the second year of life, although they are sensitive to facial information much earlier. Yet, previous research only examined whether infants are sensitive…
Descriptors: Self Concept, Infants, Human Body, Infant Behavior
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Deligianni, Fani; Senju, Atsushi; Gergely, Gyorgy; Csibra, Gergely – Developmental Psychology, 2011
The current study tested whether the purely amodal cue of contingency elicits orientation following behavior in 8-month-old infants. We presented 8-month-old infants with automated objects without human features that did or did not react contingently to the infants' fixations recorded by an eye tracker. We found that an object's occasional…
Descriptors: Infants, Social Cognition, Eye Movements, Interaction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Montirosso, Rosario; Cozzi, Patrizia; Putnam, Samuel P.; Gartstein, Maria A.; Borgatti, Renato – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2011
An Italian translation of the Infant Behavior Questionnaire-Revised (IBQ-R) was developed and evaluated with 110 infants, demonstrating satisfactory internal consistency, discriminant validity, and construct validity in the form of gender and age differences, as well as factorial integrity. Cross-cultural differences were subsequently evaluated…
Descriptors: Sociocultural Patterns, Construct Validity, Questionnaires, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Daum, Moritz M.; Prinz, Wolfgang; Aschersleben, Gisa – Infancy, 2009
In 2 experiments, the interplay of action perception and action production was investigated in 6-month-old infants. In Experiment 1, infants received 2 versions of a means-end task in counterbalanced order. In the action perception version, a preferential looking paradigm in which infants were shown an actor performing means-end behavior with an…
Descriptors: Infants, Toys, Visual Stimuli, Reinforcement
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shuwairi, Sarah M. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
Can infants use interposition and line junction cues to infer three-dimensional (3D) structure? Previous work has shown that in a task that required 4-month-olds to discriminate between static two-dimensional (2D) pictures of possible and impossible cubes, infants exhibited a spontaneous preference for displays of the impossible cube but left open…
Descriptors: Infants, Cues, Visual Discrimination, Visual Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Goldsmith, Jo; Cowen, Helena – Journal of Child Psychotherapy, 2011
This article aims to demonstrate that it is not only the mind that needs to have the capacity to hold and transform, but also the body. Fordham's concept of the "primary self" emphasises the unity between the body and emotional states in infancy. The self is expressed through actions that bring the infant into contact with the mother and the…
Descriptors: Siblings, Sexual Abuse, Mothers, Eating Disorders
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  21  |  22  |  23  |  24  |  25  |  26  |  27  |  28  |  29  |  ...  |  119