NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
MacArthur Communicative…6
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Showing 181 to 195 of 227 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Carver, Leslie J.; Meltzoff, Andrew N.; Dawson, Geraldine – Developmental Science, 2006
We measured infants' recognition of familiar and unfamiliar 3-D objects and their 2-D representations using event-related potentials (ERPs). Infants differentiated familiar from unfamiliar objects when viewing them in both two and three dimensions. However, differentiation between the familiar and novel objects occurred more quickly when infants…
Descriptors: Novelty (Stimulus Dimension), Infants, Cognitive Processes, Diagnostic Tests
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hollich, George – Language and Speech, 2006
This paper provides three representative examples that highlight the ways in which procedures can be combined to study interactions across traditional domains of study: segmentation, word learning, and grammar. The first section uses visual familiarization prior to the Headturn Preference Procedure to demonstrate that synchronized visual…
Descriptors: Sentences, Infants, Auditory Perception, Grammar
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Blaga, Otilia M.; Colombo, John – Developmental Psychology, 2006
Young infants have repeatedly been shown to be slower than older infants to shift fixation from a midline stimulus to a peripheral stimulus. This is generally thought to reflect maturation of the neural substrates that mediate the disengagement of attention, but this developmental difference may also be attributable to young infants' slower…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Infants, Attention Control, Dimensional Preference
Hertsgaard, L.; Bauer, P. J. – 1991
In two experiments, the ability of children younger than 20 months to engage in delayed ordered recall was investigated. In the first experiment, 13- and 16-month-old children were presented with 2-step event sequences and tested for recall, first, immediately following the event and second, after a one-week delay. Sequences were novel-causal,…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Familiarity, Infants, Long Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Wentworth, Naomi; Haith, Marshall M. – Developmental Psychology, 1992
A modified Visual Expectation Paradigm assessed the role of picture content in the spatiotemporal expectations of 80 infants. Stable picture content information facilitated formation of expectations about when and where pictures would appear. Two month olds' reactions were consistently slower than those of three month olds.(LB)
Descriptors: Expectation, Familiarity, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Nelson, Charles A.; And Others – Developmental Psychology, 1993
Used event-related potentials to examine infants' ability to form representations of stimuli presented in a haptic modality and to then recognize these stimuli as familiar when the stimuli were subsequently presented in a visual modality. Found that in certain conditions infants encoded the haptically familiarized object, then transferred their…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Familiarity, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bauer, Patricia J.; Hertsgaard, Louise A. – Child Development, 1993
Results of 3 experiments indicated that 13.5- and 16.5-month-old children recalled multiple sequences after a 1-week delay. Without cues, the recall of 16.5-month olds was facilitated by familiarity and by enabling relations; only enabling relations aided the 13.5-month olds' recall. With verbal cues, the recall of 13.5- and 16.5-month olds was…
Descriptors: Age Differences, Familiarity, Infants, Long Term Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Kahana-Kalman, Ronit; Walker-Andrews, Arlene S. – Child Development, 2001
Investigated the role of person familiarity in 3.5-month-olds' ability to recognize emotional expressions. Found that when more contextual information such as person familiarity was available, infants as young as 3.5 months recognized happy and sad expressions. Findings suggest that in early stages, infants are sensitive to contextual information…
Descriptors: Emotional Experience, Emotional Response, Facial Expressions, Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Montague, Diane P. F.; Walker-Andrews, Arlene S. – Child Development, 2002
Explored the effect of person familiarity and parental involvement on 3.5-month-old infants' sensitivity to the dynamic emotion expressions of others. Found that infants looked differentially at mothers' expressions but not at those of fathers or unfamiliar adults, and that parent-child involvement significantly influenced infants' developing…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Facial Expressions, Familiarity, Fathers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bauer, Patricia J.; Thal, Donna J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 1990
Elicited imitation was used to assess 21-month-olds' recall of familiar-canonical, familiar-reversed, novel-causal, and novel-arbitrary event sequences. Reversed sequences were reproduced in modeled and corrected canonical order; other sequences were reproduced in modeled order. (BC)
Descriptors: Familiarity, Imitation, Infant Behavior, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Bauer, Patricia J. – American Psychologist, 1996
Reviews research that demonstrates that children in the one- to two-year age range are able to remember specific events over weeks and months. Recall is influenced by what young children are asked to remember, the number of times they experience events, and the availability of cues or reminders. (SLD)
Descriptors: Child Development, Cues, Familiarity, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Hunnius, Sabine; Geuze, Reint H. – Infancy, 2004
The characteristics of scanning patterns between the ages of 6 and 26 weeks were investigated through repeated assessments of 10 infants. Eye movements were recorded using a corneal-reflection system while the infants looked at 2 dynamic stimuli: the naturally moving face of their mother and an abstract stimulus. Results indicated that the way…
Descriptors: Video Technology, Visual Stimuli, Mothers, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Newman, Rochelle S. – Developmental Psychology, 2005
This study examined infants' abilities to separate speech from different talkers and to recognize a familiar word (the infant's own name) in the context of noise. In 4 experiments, infants heard repetitions of either their names or unfamiliar names in the presence of background babble. Five-month-old infants listened longer to their names when the…
Descriptors: Infants, Attention Span, Acoustics, Recognition (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kobayashi, Tessei; Hiraki, Kazuo; Mugitani, Ryoko; Hasegawa, Toshikazu – Cognition, 2004
Recent studies using a violation-of-expectation task suggest that preverbal infants are capable of recognizing basic arithmetical operations involving visual objects. There is still debate, however, over whether their performance is based on any expectation of the arithmetical operations, or on a general perceptual tendency to prefer visually…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Arithmetic, Infants, Learning Modalities
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Thiessen, Erik D.; Hill, Emily A.; Saffran, Jenny R. – Infancy, 2005
There are reasons to believe that infant-directed (ID) speech may make language acquisition easier for infants. However, the effects of ID speech on infants' learning remain poorly understood. The experiments reported here assess whether ID speech facilitates word segmentation from fluent speech. One group of infants heard a set of nonsense…
Descriptors: Sentences, Intonation, Infants, Language Acquisition
Pages: 1  |  ...  |  6  |  7  |  8  |  9  |  10  |  11  |  12  |  13  |  14  |  15  |  16