NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 17 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Bergmann, Christina; Cristia, Alejandrina – Developmental Science, 2016
Infants start learning words, the building blocks of language, at least by 6 months. To do so, they must be able to extract the phonological form of words from running speech. A rich literature has investigated this process, termed word segmentation. We addressed the fundamental question of how infants of different ages segment words from their…
Descriptors: Infants, Meta Analysis, Native Language, Stimuli
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Yang, Jiale; Kanazawa, So; Yamaguchi, Masami K.; Kuriki, Ichiro – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2013
The current study examined color constancy in infants using a familiarization paradigm. We first obtained isoluminance in each infant as defined by the minimum motion paradigm and used these data to control the luminance of stimuli in the main experiments. In the familiarization phase of the main experiment, two identical smiling face patterns…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Infants, Models, Color
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kovack-Lesh, Kristine A.; Oakes, Lisa M.; McMurray, Bob – Infancy, 2012
We examined how infants' categorization is jointly influenced by previous experience and how much they shift their gaze back and forth between stimuli. Extending previous findings reported by K. A. Kovack-Lesh, J. S. Horst, and L. M. Oakes (2008), we found that 4-month-old infants' (N = 122) learning of the exclusive category of "cats" was related…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Classification, Infants, Attention
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Moore, David S.; Johnson, Scott P. – Infancy, 2011
Mental rotation involves transforming a mental image of an object so as to accurately predict how the object would look if it were rotated in space. This study examined mental rotation in male and female 3-month-olds, using the stimuli and paradigm developed by Moore and Johnson (2008). Infants were habituated to a video of a three-dimensional…
Descriptors: Spatial Ability, Visualization, Stimuli, Infants
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Althaus, Nadja; Mareschal, Denis – Child Development, 2012
This article presents an eye-tracking study using a novel combination of visual saliency maps and "area-of-interest" analyses to explore online feature extraction during category learning in infants. Category learning in 12-month-olds (N = 22) involved a transition from looking at high-saliency image regions to looking at more…
Descriptors: Maps, Classification, Infants, Eye Movements
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gonzalez-Gomez, Nayeli; Nazzi, Thierry – Journal of Speech, Language, and Hearing Research, 2013
Purpose: In this study, the authors explored whether French-learning infants use nonadjacent phonotactic regularities in their native language, which they learn between the ages of 7 and 10 months, to segment words from fluent speech. Method: Two groups of 20 French-learning infants were tested using the head-turn preference procedure at 10 and 13…
Descriptors: Prior Learning, Infants, French, Phonology
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Fair, Joseph; Flom, Ross; Jones, Jacob; Martin, Justin – Child Development, 2012
Six-month-olds reliably discriminate different monkey and human faces whereas 9-month-olds only discriminate different human faces. It is often falsely assumed that perceptual narrowing reflects a permanent change in perceptual abilities. In 3 experiments, ninety-six 12-month-olds' discrimination of unfamiliar monkey faces was examined. Following…
Descriptors: Primatology, Infants, Human Body, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Anaki, David; Bentin, Shlomo – Cognition, 2009
It is well established that faces, in contrast to objects, are categorized as fast or faster at the individual level (e.g., Bill Clinton) than at the basic-level (e.g., human face). This subordinate-shift from basic-level categorization has been considered an outcome of visual expertise with processing faces. However, in the present study we found…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Semantics, Familiarity, Children
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shinskey, Jeanne L.; Munakata, Yuko – Developmental Science, 2010
Novelty seeking is viewed as adaptive, and novelty preferences in infancy predict cognitive performance into adulthood. Yet 7-month-olds prefer familiar stimuli to novel ones when searching for hidden objects, in contrast to their strong novelty preferences with visible objects (Shinskey & Munakata, 2005). According to a graded representations…
Descriptors: Object Permanence, Stimuli, Familiarity, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Snyder, Kelly A.; Garza, John; Zolot, Liza; Kresse, Anna – Infancy, 2010
Electrophysiological work in nonhuman primates has established the existence of multiple types of signals in the temporal lobe that contribute to recognition memory, including information regarding a stimulus's relative novelty, familiarity, and recency of occurrence. We used high-density event-related potentials (ERPs) to examine whether young…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Familiarity, Infants, Recognition (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Clearfield, Melissa W.; Westfahl, Shannon May-Comyns – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2006
Previous research suggests that infants can calculate simple arithmetic (Wynn, 1992). This study sought to replicate this finding and examine how familiarization may influence response. Experiment 1 confirmed that 3- to 5-month-old infants looked longer at an incorrect outcome (1 + 1 = 1) when no familiarization trials were presented. Experiment 2…
Descriptors: Infants, Cognitive Processes, Arithmetic, Familiarity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Wiebe, Sandra A.; Cheatham, Carol L.; Lukowski, Angela F.; Haight, Jennifer C.; Muehleck, Abigail J.; Bauer, Patricia J. – Infancy, 2006
Detection of novelty is an important cognitive ability early in development, when infants must learn a great deal about their world. Work with adults has identified networks of brain areas involved in novelty detection; this study investigated electro-physiological correlates of detection of novelty and recognition of familiarity in 9-month-old…
Descriptors: Infants, Brain, Change, Recognition (Psychology)
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cleveland, Allison; Schug, Mariah; Striano, Tricia – Infant and Child Development, 2007
We examined the effects of joint attention for object learning in 5- and 7-month-old infants. Infants interacted with an adult social partner who taught them about a novel toy in two conditions. In the "Joint Attention" condition, the adult spoke about the toy while alternating gaze between the infant and the toy, while in the…
Descriptors: Infants, Interpersonal Relationship, Toys, Interaction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Blaga, Otilia M.; Colombo, John – Developmental Psychology, 2006
Young infants have repeatedly been shown to be slower than older infants to shift fixation from a midline stimulus to a peripheral stimulus. This is generally thought to reflect maturation of the neural substrates that mediate the disengagement of attention, but this developmental difference may also be attributable to young infants' slower…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Infants, Attention Control, Dimensional Preference
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Shultz, Thomas R.; Cohen, Leslie B. – Infancy, 2004
We used an encoder version of cascade correlation to simulate Younger and Cohen's (1983, 1986) finding that 10-month-olds recover attention on the basis of correlations among stimulus features, but 4- and 7-month-olds recover attention on the basis of stimulus features. We captured these effects by varying the score threshold parameter in cascade…
Descriptors: Infants, Learning, Age Differences, Attention
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1  |  2