Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 41 |
Descriptor
Experimental Psychology | 54 |
Experiments | 54 |
Visual Stimuli | 54 |
Visual Perception | 16 |
Spatial Ability | 13 |
Undergraduate Students | 11 |
Attention | 10 |
Cognitive Processes | 10 |
Cues | 10 |
Memory | 10 |
Research Methodology | 10 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Angele, Bernhard | 2 |
Henderson, John M. | 2 |
Humphreys, Glyn W. | 2 |
Kanazawa, So | 2 |
Kunde, Wilfried | 2 |
Otsuka, Yumiko | 2 |
Rayner, Keith | 2 |
Yamaguchi, Masami K. | 2 |
Ackroyd, Katie | 1 |
Alvarez, George A. | 1 |
Anzures, Gizelle | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 45 |
Reports - Research | 41 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Reports - Descriptive | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 17 |
Early Childhood Education | 4 |
Adult Education | 2 |
Postsecondary Education | 2 |
High Schools | 1 |
Audience
Location
California | 2 |
Germany | 2 |
Florida | 1 |
France | 1 |
Illinois | 1 |
Iowa | 1 |
Michigan | 1 |
Minnesota | 1 |
Netherlands | 1 |
North Carolina | 1 |
South Carolina | 1 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Edinburgh Handedness Inventory | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Brady, Timothy F.; Alvarez, George A. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
A central question for models of visual working memory is whether the number of objects people can remember depends on object complexity. Some influential "slot" models of working memory capacity suggest that people always represent 3-4 objects and that only the fidelity with which these objects are represented is affected by object…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Experiments, Short Term Memory, Visual Perception
Torres, Marta N.; Rodríguez, Clara A.; Chamizo, V. D.; Mackintosh, N. J. – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2014
Rats were trained in a triangular-shaped pool to find a hidden platform, whose location was defined in terms of two sources of information, a landmark outside the pool and a particular corner of the pool. Subsequent test trials without the platform pitted these two sources of information against one another. In Experiment 1 this test revealed a…
Descriptors: Animals, Animal Behavior, Experimental Psychology, Experiments
Won, Bo-Yeong; Jiang, Yuhong V. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2015
Recent empirical and theoretical work has depicted a close relationship between visual attention and visual working memory. For example, rehearsal in spatial working memory depends on spatial attention, whereas adding a secondary spatial working memory task impairs attentional deployment in visual search. These findings have led to the proposal…
Descriptors: Experimental Psychology, Experiments, Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability
Otsuka, Yumiko; Hill, Harold C. H.; Kanazawa, So; Yamaguchi, Masami K.; Spehar, Branka – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
We examined the ability of young infants (3- and 4-month-olds) to detect faces in the two-tone images often referred to as Mooney faces. In Experiment 1, this performance was examined in conditions of high and low visibility of local features and with either the presence or absence of the outer head contour. We found that regardless of the…
Descriptors: Infants, Motion, Perception, Identification
Janczyk, Markus; Pfister, Roland; Wallmeier, Gloria; Kunde, Wilfried – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2014
Psychological research has documented again and again marked performance decrements whenever humans perform 2 or more tasks at the same time. In fact, the available evidence seems to suggest that any type of behavior is subject to such limitations. The present experiments employed the psychological refractory period (PRP) paradigm to identify a…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Task Analysis, Experimental Psychology, Experiments
Luke, Steven G.; Nuthmann, Antje; Henderson, John M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2013
The present study used the stimulus onset delay paradigm to investigate eye movement control in reading and in scene viewing in a within-participants design. Short onset delays (0, 25, 50, 200, and 350 ms) were chosen to simulate the type of natural processing difficulty encountered in reading and scene viewing. Fixation duration increased…
Descriptors: Individual Differences, Human Body, Attention, Models
Gul, Amara; Humphreys, Glyn W. – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2015
Congruency effects were examined using a manual response version of the Stroop task in which the relationship between the colour word and its hue on incongruent trials was either kept constant or varied randomly across different pairings within the stimulus set. Congruency effects were increased in the condition where the incongruent hue-word…
Descriptors: Experiments, Psychological Testing, Perceptual Development, Perception Tests
Frischen, Alexandra; Ferrey, Anne E.; Burt, Dustin H. R.; Pistchik, Meghan; Fenske, Mark J. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2012
Affective evaluations of previously ignored visual stimuli are more negative than those of novel items or prior targets of attention or response. This has been taken as evidence that inhibition has negative affective consequences. But inhibition could act instead to attenuate or "neutralize" preexisting affective salience, predicting opposite…
Descriptors: Evidence, Visual Stimuli, Inhibition, Cognitive Processes
Angele, Bernhard; Rayner, Keith – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2013
One of the words that readers of English skip most often is the definite article "the". Most accounts of reading assume that in order for a reader to skip a word, it must have received some lexical processing. The definite article is skipped so regularly, however, that the oculomotor system might have learned to skip the letter string…
Descriptors: Form Classes (Languages), Sentences, Verbs, Language Processing
Boulton, Michael J. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Hostile attribution bias (HAB) has been found to characterize aggressive children. Watching prosocial media has been shown to have positive effects on children, and the general learning model has been used to account for these observations. This study tested the hypotheses derived from this theory that exposure to playful fighting would lead to a…
Descriptors: Play, Teacher Attitudes, Intervention, Aggression
Anzures, Gizelle; Wheeler, Andrea; Quinn, Paul C.; Pascalis, Olivier; Slater, Alan M.; Heron-Delaney, Michelle; Tanaka, James W.; Lee, Kang – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Perceptual narrowing in the visual, auditory, and multisensory domains has its developmental origins during infancy. The current study shows that experimentally induced experience can reverse the effects of perceptual narrowing on infants' visual recognition memory of other-race faces. Caucasian 8- to 10-month-olds who could not discriminate…
Descriptors: Females, Infants, Recognition (Psychology), Whites
Noles, Nicholaus S.; Gelman, Susan A. – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
The goal of this study was to evaluate the claim that young children display preferences for auditory stimuli over visual stimuli. This study was motivated by concerns that the visual stimuli employed in prior studies were considerably more complex and less distinctive than the competing auditory stimuli, resulting in an illusory preference for…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Cues, Auditory Stimuli, Preschool Children
Munzer, Stefan; Zimmer, Hubert D.; Baus, Jorg – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2012
Current GPS-based mobile navigation assistance systems support wayfinding, but they do not support learning about the spatial configuration of an environment. The present study examined effects of visual presentation modes for navigation assistance on wayfinding accuracy, route learning, and configural learning. Participants (high-school students)…
Descriptors: Computer Assisted Instruction, Maps, Short Term Memory, Spatial Ability
Kobayashi, Megumi; Otsuka, Yumiko; Nakato, Emi; Kanazawa, So; Yamaguchi, Masami K.; Kakigi, Ryusuke – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Arcimboldo images induce the perception of faces when shown upright despite the fact that only nonfacial objects such as vegetables and fruits are painted. In the current study, we examined whether infants recognize a face in the Arcimboldo images by using the preferential looking technique and near-infrared spectroscopy (NIRS). In the first…
Descriptors: Metabolism, Infants, Males, Experimental Psychology
La Heij, Wido; Boelens, Harrie – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
Young children are slower in naming the color of a meaningful picture than in naming the color of an abstract form (Stroop-like color-object interference). The current experiments tested an executive control account of this phenomenon. First, color-object interference was observed in 6- and 8-year-olds but not in 12- and 16-year-olds (Experiment…
Descriptors: Visual Stimuli, Color, Observation, Age Differences