Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 1 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 17 |
Descriptor
Evolution | 17 |
Experiments | 17 |
Cognitive Processes | 4 |
Biology | 3 |
College Students | 3 |
Foreign Countries | 3 |
Science Education | 3 |
Animals | 2 |
Behavioral Science Research | 2 |
Classification | 2 |
Cognitive Development | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 17 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Descriptive | 6 |
Reports - Evaluative | 4 |
Guides - Classroom - Teacher | 1 |
Education Level
Higher Education | 4 |
Secondary Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Elementary Secondary Education | 1 |
Postsecondary Education | 1 |
Audience
Teachers | 2 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Koopman, Sarah E.; Mahon, Bradford Z.; Cantlon, Jessica F. – Cognitive Science, 2017
Language and culture endow humans with access to conceptual information that far exceeds any which could be accessed by a non-human animal. Yet, it is possible that, even without language or specific experiences, non-human animals represent and infer some aspects of similarity relations between objects in the same way as humans. Here, we show that…
Descriptors: Evolution, Animals, Discrimination Learning, Inferences
Proponents of Creationism but Not Proponents of Evolution Frame the Origins Debate in Terms of Proof
Barnes, Ralph M.; Church, Rebecca A. – Science & Education, 2013
In Study 1, 72 internet documents containing creationism, ID (intelligent design), or evolution content were selected for analysis. All instances of proof cognates (the word "proof" and related terms such as "proven", "disproof", etc.) contained within these documents were identified and labeled in terms of the manner in which the terms were used.…
Descriptors: Evolution, Internet, Qualitative Research, Data Analysis
Green, Jennifer H.; Koza, Anna; Moshynets, Olena; Pajor, Radoslaw; Ritchie, Margaret R.; Spiers, Andrew J. – Journal of Biological Education, 2011
Understanding evolutionary mechanisms is fundamental to a balanced biological education, yet practical demonstrations are rarely considered. In this paper we describe a bacterial liquid microcosm which can be used to demonstrate aspects of evolution, namely adaptive radiation, niche colonisation and competitive fitness. In microcosms inoculated…
Descriptors: Evolution, Molecular Biology, Biology, Science Instruction
Lightman, Bernard – Science & Education, 2012
Evolution was a difficult topic to tackle when writing books for the young in the wake of the controversies over Darwin's "Origin of Species." Authors who wrote about evolution for the young experimented with different ways of making the complex concepts of evolutionary theory accessible and less controversial. Many authors depicted presented…
Descriptors: Evolution, Theories, Science Education History, Religion
Plunkett, Andrea D.; Yampolsky, Lev Y. – American Biology Teacher, 2010
We propose an experimental model suitable for demonstrating allele frequency change in Drosophila melanogaster populations caused by selection against an easily scorable conditional lethal, namely recessive flightless alleles such as apterous and vestigial. Homozygotes for these alleles are excluded from reproduction because the food source used…
Descriptors: Evolution, Entomology, Birth, Biology
Wendel, Paul J. – Science & Education, 2011
In a regional young-earth creationist museum, objects are presented as if they speak for themselves, purportedly embodying proof that the earth is less than 10,000 years old, that humans have lived on earth throughout its history, and that dinosaurs and humans lived simultaneously. In public lectures, tours, and displays, museum associates…
Descriptors: Observation, Paleontology, Museums, Creationism
Kroneisen, Meike; Erdfelder, Edgar – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2011
Nairne, Thompson, and Pandeirada (2007) discovered a strong and rather general memory advantage for word material processed in a survival-related context. One possible explanation of this effect conceives survival processing as a special form of encoding: Nature specifically "tuned" our memory systems to process and remember…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Language Processing, Replication (Evaluation), Evolution
Nairne, James S.; Pandeirada, Josefa N. S. – Cognitive Psychology, 2010
Evolutionary psychologists often propose that humans carry around "stone-age" brains, along with a toolkit of cognitive adaptations designed originally to solve hunter-gatherer problems. This perspective predicts that optimal cognitive performance might sometimes be induced by ancestrally-based problems, those present in ancestral environments,…
Descriptors: Psychologists, Memory, Urban Environment, Prediction
Weiss, Matthias; Dreesmann, Daniel C. – Universal Journal of Educational Research, 2014
Biological evolution still lacks representation in school, especially below high school level. In order to find new implications that could help achieve significant improvement in teaching evolution, twelve expert interviews with both renowned researchers and experienced science teachers were conducted. Results of the comparison between the…
Descriptors: Foreign Countries, Evolution, Science Instruction, Scientists
Scott-Phillips, Thomas C.; Kirby, Simon; Ritchie, Graham R. S. – Cognition, 2009
A unique hallmark of human language is that it uses signals that are both learnt and symbolic. The emergence of such signals was therefore a defining event in human cognitive evolution, yet very little is known about how such a process occurs. Previous work provides some insights on how meaning can become attached to form, but a more foundational…
Descriptors: Experiments, Cognitive Processes, Cognitive Development, Games
Lattal, Kennon A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2008
Evidence of how behavioral research and technology have evolved together abounds in the history of the "Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior" ("JEAB"). Technology from outside the discipline (exogenous) from such disciplines as electronics and computer science has been adapted for use in behavioral research. Technology from within the…
Descriptors: Periodicals, Behavioral Science Research, Technology, Evolution
Novick, Laura R.; Catley, Kefyn M. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Applied, 2007
Cladograms, hierarchical diagrams depicting evolutionary histories among (groups of) species, are commonly drawn in 2 informationally equivalent formats--tree and ladder. The authors hypothesize that these formats are not computationally equivalent because the Gestalt principle of good continuation obscures the hierarchical structure of ladders.…
Descriptors: Evolution, Biology, Educational Change, College Students
Becklin, Katie M. – American Biology Teacher, 2008
Plants and insects share a long evolutionary history characterized by relationships that affect individual, population, and community dynamics. Plant-herbivore interactions are a prominent feature of this evolutionary history; it is by plant-herbivore interactions that energy is transferred from primary producers to the rest of the food web. Not…
Descriptors: Evolution, Entomology, Ecology, Biodiversity
Garrod, Simon; Fay, Nicolas; Lee, John; Oberlander, Jon; MacLeod, Tracy – Cognitive Science, 2007
It has been suggested that iconic graphical signs evolve into symbolic graphical signs through repeated usage. This article reports a series of interactive graphical communication experiments using a "pictionary" task to establish the conditions under which the evolution might occur. Experiment 1 rules out a simple repetition based account in…
Descriptors: Interaction, Foreign Countries, Internet, Orthographic Symbols
Nairne, James S.; Thompson, Sarah R.; Pandeirada, Josefa N. S. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2007
The authors investigated the idea that memory systems might have evolved to help us remember fitness-relevant information--specifically, information relevant to survival. In 4 incidental learning experiments, people were asked to rate common nouns for their survival relevance (e.g., in securing food, water, or protection from predators); in…
Descriptors: Nouns, Incidental Learning, Retention (Psychology), Experiments
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2