Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 0 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 0 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 9 |
Descriptor
Experimental Psychology | 9 |
Experiments | 9 |
Age Differences | 4 |
Child Psychology | 4 |
Children | 4 |
Task Analysis | 4 |
Adults | 3 |
Comparative Analysis | 3 |
Teaching Methods | 3 |
Cognitive Processes | 2 |
Elementary School Students | 2 |
More ▼ |
Source
Journal of Experimental Child… | 6 |
Cognition | 1 |
Journal of Experimental… | 1 |
Psicologica: International… | 1 |
Author
Apperly, Ian A. | 1 |
Beck, Sarah R. | 1 |
Beilei, Li | 1 |
Beisert, Miriam | 1 |
Castles, Anne | 1 |
Chappell, Jackie | 1 |
Congying, Sun | 1 |
Cutting, Nicola | 1 |
Evans, Angela D. | 1 |
Fu, Genyue | 1 |
Guthrie, Carlie | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 9 |
Reports - Research | 7 |
Reports - Evaluative | 2 |
Education Level
Elementary Education | 9 |
Early Childhood Education | 4 |
Grade 3 | 2 |
Grade 5 | 2 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 4 | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Location
United Kingdom | 1 |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Fu, Genyue; Evans, Angela D.; Xu, Fen; Lee, Kang – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
This study investigated whether young children make strategic decisions about whether to lie to conceal a transgression based on the lie recipient's knowledge. In Experiment 1, 168 3- to 5-year-olds were asked not to peek at the toy in the experimenter's absence, and the majority of children peeked. Children were questioned about their…
Descriptors: Preschool Children, Strategic Planning, Experiments, Age Differences
Kenward, Ben – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Over-imitation, which is common in children, is the imitation of elements of an action sequence that are clearly unnecessary for reaching the final goal. A variety of cognitive mechanisms have been proposed to explain this phenomenon. Here, 48 3- and 5-year-olds together with a puppet observed an adult demonstrate instrumental tasks that included…
Descriptors: Conferences (Gatherings), Puppetry, Imitation, Critical Thinking
Schlottmann, Anne; Harman, Rachel M.; Paine, Julie – Psicologica: International Journal of Methodology and Experimental Psychology, 2012
Under the normative Expected Value (EV) model, multiple outcomes are additive, but in everyday worth judgement intuitive averaging prevails. Young children also use averaging in EV judgements, leading to a disordinal, crossover violation of utility when children average the part worths of simple gambles involving independent events (Schlottmann,…
Descriptors: Task Analysis, Models, Children, Age Differences
Beck, Sarah R.; Apperly, Ian A.; Chappell, Jackie; Guthrie, Carlie; Cutting, Nicola – Cognition, 2011
Tool making evidences intelligent, flexible thinking. In Experiment 1, we confirmed that 4- to 7-year-olds chose a hook tool to retrieve a bucket from a tube. In Experiment 2, 3- to 5-year-olds consistently failed to innovate a simple hook tool. Eight-year-olds performed at mature levels. In contrast, making a tool following demonstration was easy…
Descriptors: Experiments, Children, Thinking Skills, Age Differences
Beisert, Miriam; Massen, Cristina; Prinz, Wolfgang – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Human Perception and Performance, 2010
In tool use, a transformation rule defines the relation between an operating movement and its distal effect. This rule is determined by the tool structure and requires no explicit definition. The present study investigates how humans represent and apply compatible and incompatible transformation rules in tool use. In Experiment 1, participants had…
Descriptors: Elementary School Students, Experiments, Models, Motion
Vida, Mark D.; Maurer, Daphne – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Adults use eye contact as a cue to the mental and emotional states of others. Here, we examined developmental changes in the ability to discriminate between eye contact and averted gaze. Children (6-, 8-, 10-, and 14-year-olds) and adults (n=18/age) viewed photographs of a model fixating the center of a camera lens and a series of positions to the…
Descriptors: Photography, Cues, Nonverbal Communication, Children
Wang, Hua-Chen; Castles, Anne; Nickels, Lyndsey; Nation, Kate – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2011
The self-teaching hypothesis proposes that orthographic learning takes place via phonological decoding in meaningful texts, that is, in context. Context is proposed to be important in learning to read, especially when decoding is only partial. However, little research has directly explored this hypothesis. The current study looked at the effect of…
Descriptors: Spelling, Phonetic Transcription, Reading Comprehension, Vocabulary Development
Peng, Peng; Congying, Sun; Beilei, Li; Sha, Tao – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2012
Children with mathematics difficulties suffer from working memory deficits. This study investigated the deficit profile of phonological storage and executive functions in working memory among children with mathematics difficulties. Based on multiple instruments and two assessment points, 68 children were screened out of 805 fifth graders. Of these…
Descriptors: Reading Difficulties, Learning Disabilities, Inhibition, Short Term Memory
Matthews, Percival; Rittle-Johnson, Bethany – Journal of Experimental Child Psychology, 2009
Explaining new ideas to oneself can promote learning and transfer, but questions remain about how to maximize the pedagogical value of self-explanations. This study investigated how type of instruction affected self-explanation quality and subsequent learning outcomes for second- through fifth-grade children learning to solve mathematical…
Descriptors: Grade 5, Mathematics Instruction, Teaching Methods, Comparative Analysis