NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing 1 to 15 of 28 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Vallesi, Antonino; Lozano, Violeta N.; Correa, Angel – Cognition, 2013
Preparation over time is a ubiquitous capacity which implies decreasing uncertainty about when critical events will occur. This capacity is usually studied with the variable foreperiod paradigm, which consists in the random variation of the time interval (foreperiod) between a warning stimulus and a target. With this paradigm, response time (RT)…
Descriptors: Models, Intervals, Reaction Time, Prediction
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Jimenez-Gomez, Corina; Shahan, Timothy A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2012
An extensive body of research using concurrent-chains schedules of reinforcement has shown that choice for one of two differentially valued food-associated stimuli is dependent upon the overall temporal context in which those stimuli are embedded. The present experiments examined whether the concurrent chains procedure was useful for the study of…
Descriptors: Stimuli, Reinforcement, Intervals, Food
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kehoe, E. James; Ludvig, Elliot A.; Sutton, Richard S. – Learning & Memory, 2010
Using interstimulus intervals (ISIs) of 125, 250, and 500 msec in trace conditioning of the rabbit nictitating membrane response, the offset times and durations of conditioned responses (CRs) were collected along with onset and peak latencies. All measures were proportional to the ISI, but only onset and peak latencies conformed to the criterion…
Descriptors: Intervals, Conditioning, Animals, Stimuli
Cowie, Sarah; Davison, Michael; Elliffe, Douglas – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
It has long been understood that food deliveries may act as signals of future food location, and not only as strengtheners of prefood responding as the law of effect suggests. Recent research has taken this idea further--the main effect of food deliveries, or other "reinforcers", may be signaling rather than strengthening. The present experiment…
Descriptors: Conditioning, Stimuli, Reinforcement, Animals
White, K. Geoffrey; Brown, Glenn S. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2011
Pigeons performed a delayed matching-to-sample task in which large or small reinforcers for correct remembering were signaled during the retention interval. Accuracy was low when small reinforcers were signaled, and high when large reinforcers were signaled (the signaled magnitude effect). When the reinforcer-size cue was switched from small to…
Descriptors: Animals, Reinforcement, Accuracy, Memory
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Greer, R. Douglas; Pistoljevic, Nirvana; Cahill, Claire; Du, Lin – Analysis of Verbal Behavior, 2011
We used a delayed non-concurrent pre- and post-intervention probe design to test the effects of a voice conditioning protocol (VCP) with 3 preschoolers with autism on (a) rate of acquisition of listener curricular objectives, (b) observing voices and the presence of adults across 3 settings, (c) selecting to listen to adults tell stories in free…
Descriptors: Intervention, Adults, Behavior Problems, Story Telling
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
De la Casa, L. G.; Marquez, R.; Lubow, R. E. – Learning and Motivation, 2009
A long delay inserted between conditioning and test phases of a 3-stage Latent Inhibition (LI) procedure produces differential effects on LI depending on the delay context. Thus, enhanced LI has been obtained when the delay is spent in a context that is different from the remaining experimental contexts, but not when it is the same. The present…
Descriptors: Intervals, Conditioning, Inhibition, Retention (Psychology)
Tanno, Takayuki; Silberberg, Alan; Sakagami, Takayuki – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2009
Food-deprived rats in Experiment 1 responded to one of two tandem schedules that were, with equal probability, associated with a sample lever. The tandem schedules' initial links were different random-interval schedules. Their values were adjusted to approximate equality in time to completing each tandem schedule's response requirements. The…
Descriptors: Intervals, Probability, Animals, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Kimura, Ryoichi; Silva, Alcino J.; Ohno, Masuo – Learning & Memory, 2008
Accumulating evidence indicates the key role of [alpha]-calcium/calmodulin-dependent protein kinase II ([alpha]CaMKII) in synaptic plasticity and learning, but it remains unclear how this kinase participates in the processing of memory extinction. Here, we investigated the mechanism by which [alpha]CaMKII may mediate extinction by using…
Descriptors: Intervals, Memory, Brain, Fear
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Williams, Douglas A.; Chubala, Chrissy M.; Mather, Amber A.; Johns, Kenneth W. – Learning and Motivation, 2009
Appetitive contextual excitation supported by intertrial unconditioned stimuli was more easily overcome by timed conditioned responding in rats using quiet (Experiment 1) rather than noisy (Experiment 2) food pellet deliveries. Head-entry responding in acquisition peaked above the contextual baseline when pellet delivery occurred 10, 30, 60, or 90…
Descriptors: Cues, Intervals, Reaction Time, Food
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Escobar, Rogelio; Bruner, Carlos A. – Journal of the Experimental Analysis of Behavior, 2007
The acquisition of lever pressing by rats and the occurrence of unreinforced presses at a location different from that of the reinforced response were studied using different delays of reinforcement. An experimental chamber containing seven identical adjoining levers was used. Only presses on the central (operative) lever produced food pellets.…
Descriptors: Logical Thinking, Reinforcement, Intervals, Animals
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Pellegrini, Santiago; Papini, Mauricio R. – Learning and Motivation, 2007
Papini and Pellegrini (Papini, M. R., & Pellegrini, S. "Scaling relative incentive value in consummatory behavior." "Learning and Motivation", in press) observed that, within limits, the level of consummatory responding of rats exposed to incentive downshifts in the concentration of sucrose solutions was similar when the ratio of test/training…
Descriptors: Scaling, Incentives, Behavior, Conditioning
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Biedenkapp, Joseph C.; Rudy, Jerry W. – Learning & Memory, 2007
Contextual fear conditioning was maintained over a 15-day retention interval suggesting no forgetting of the conditioning experience. However, a more subtle generalization test revealed that, as the retention interval increased, rats showed enhanced generalized fear to an altered context. Preexposure to the training context prior to conditioning,…
Descriptors: Intervals, Conditioning, Fear, Generalization
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Baldi, Elisabetta; Mariottini, Chiara; Bucherelli, Corrado – Learning & Memory, 2007
The nucleus basalis magnocellularis (NBM) is known to be involved in the memorization of several conditioned responses. To investigate the role of the NBM in fear conditioning memorization, this neural site was subjected to fully reversible tetrodotoxin (TTX) inactivation during consolidation in adult male Wistar rats that had undergone fear…
Descriptors: Auditory Stimuli, Intervals, Conditioning, Integrity
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Papini, Mauricio R.; Pellegrini, Santiago – Learning and Motivation, 2006
Surprising downshifts from more preferred (training incentive) to less preferred incentives (test incentive) are usually accompanied by emotional activation and suppression of conditioned behavior in rats. Two experiments were designed to determine whether consummatory behavior is similarly affected by downshifts of equal proportions. Within…
Descriptors: Scaling, Incentives, Behavior, Conditioning
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1  |  2