NotesFAQContact Us
Collection
Advanced
Search Tips
Showing all 7 results Save | Export
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Roberson, Debi; Kikutani, Mariko; Doge, Paula; Whitaker, Lydia; Majid, Asifa – Cognition, 2012
Three studies investigated developmental changes in facial expression processing, between 3 years-of-age and adulthood. For adults and older children, the addition of sunglasses to upright faces caused an equivalent decrement in performance to face inversion. However, younger children showed "better" classification of expressions of faces wearing…
Descriptors: Emotional Development, Nonverbal Communication, Classification, Research
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mahajan, Neha; Wynn, Karen – Cognition, 2012
A central feature of human psychology is our pervasive tendency to divide the social world into "us" and "them". We prefer to associate with those who are similar to us over those who are different, preferentially allocate resources to similar others, and hold more positive beliefs about similar others. Here we investigate the developmental…
Descriptors: Infants, Interpersonal Attraction, Values, Cultural Influences
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Mani, Nivedita; Plunkett, Kim – Cognition, 2011
Adult word recognition is influenced by prior exposure to phonologically or semantically related words ("cup" primes "cat" or "plate") compared to unrelated words ("door"), suggesting that words are organised in the adult lexicon based on their phonological and semantic properties and that word recognition implicates not just the heard word, but…
Descriptors: Priming, Phonology, Semantics, Toddlers
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Gebuis, Titia; Gevers, Wim – Cognition, 2011
de Hevia and Spelke (de Hevia and Spelke (2009). Spontaneous mapping of number and space in adults and young children, "Cognition, 110", 198-207) investigated the mapping of number onto space. To this end, they introduced a non-symbolic flanker task. Here subjects have to bisect a line that is flanked by a 2-dot and a 9-dot array. Similar to the…
Descriptors: Cues, Cognitive Processes, Investigations, Cognitive Mapping
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Cheyne, James Allan; Carriere, Jonathan S. A.; Solman, Grayden J. F.; Smilek, Daniel – Cognition, 2011
Attention lapses resulting from reactivity to task challenges and their consequences constitute a pervasive factor affecting everyday performance errors and accidents. A bidirectional model of attention lapses (error [image omitted] attention-lapse: Cheyne, Solman, Carriere, & Smilek, 2009) argues that errors beget errors by generating attention…
Descriptors: Accidents, Learning Processes, Educational Environment, Partnerships in Education
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Goksun, Tilbe; Hirsh-Pasek, Kathy; Golinkoff, Roberta Michnick; Imai, Mutsumi; Konishi, Haruka; Okada, Hiroyuki – Cognition, 2011
To learn relational terms such as verbs and prepositions, children must first dissect and process dynamic event components. This paper investigates the way in which 8- to 14-month-old English-reared infants notice the event components, "figure" (i.e., the moving entity) and "ground" (i.e., stationary setting), in both dynamic…
Descriptors: Infants, Old English, Investigations, Experiments
Peer reviewed Peer reviewed
Direct linkDirect link
Stone, Anna – Cognition, 2008
The Burton, Bruce and Johnston [Burton, A. M., Bruce, V., & Johnston, R. A. (1990). Understanding face recognition with an interactive activation model. "British Journal of Psychology," 81, 361-380] model of person recognition proposes that representations of known persons are connected by shared semantic attributes. This predicts that priming…
Descriptors: Investigations, Semantics, Familiarity, Cognitive Processes