Publication Date
In 2025 | 1 |
Since 2024 | 2 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 7 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 20 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 102 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 930 |
Language Research | 930 |
Psycholinguistics | 373 |
Language Processing | 254 |
Language Acquisition | 234 |
Second Language Learning | 222 |
Semantics | 204 |
Linguistic Theory | 189 |
Child Language | 165 |
Memory | 156 |
Syntax | 123 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Publication Type
Education Level
Higher Education | 11 |
Postsecondary Education | 5 |
Elementary Education | 4 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Adult Basic Education | 1 |
Adult Education | 1 |
Grade 1 | 1 |
Grade 2 | 1 |
Grade 3 | 1 |
Primary Education | 1 |
Audience
Practitioners | 13 |
Researchers | 13 |
Teachers | 6 |
Students | 1 |
Location
Canada | 7 |
Japan | 6 |
United States | 5 |
Australia | 3 |
Finland | 3 |
South Korea | 3 |
Texas | 3 |
United Kingdom (Great Britain) | 3 |
Africa | 2 |
China | 2 |
France | 2 |
More ▼ |
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Elementary and Secondary… | 1 |
Assessments and Surveys
Stroop Color Word Test | 2 |
Rorschach Test | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Tzeng, Ovid J. L.; Wang, William S. Y. – American Scientist, 1983
Indicates that the way different languages reduce speech to script affects how visual information is processed in the brain, suggesting that the relation between script and speech underlying all types of writing systems plays an important part in reading behavior. Compares memory performance of native English/Chinese speakers. (JN)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Interference (Language), Language Processing, Language Research

Pearson, P. David; Fielding, Linda – Language Arts, 1982
Discusses four aspects of listening comprehension citing supportive research: (1) what is involved in listening comprehension, (2) whether it be taught, (3) how it is related to reading comprehension, and (4) what other factors affect listening comprehension. Provides recommendations for teaching. (HTH)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Language Research, Learning Theories, Listening Comprehension

Hudson, Lynne M.; And Others – Journal of Child Language, 1982
Kindergarteners without number conservation ability were found to rely on the nonlinguistic strategy of choosing the greater amount in tasks requiring the choice of more and less. Mature semantic knowledge of "more" was found to precede that of "less." (Author)
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Conservation (Concept), Kindergarten Children

Ryan, Michael P.; Hurtig, Richard R. – Discourse Processes, 1980
Delineates a study showing that jurors arriving at different verdicts differ in the way they compute verdicts from a common evidence base rather than in the structure of their evidence bases. Concludes that the legal system's distinction between evidence comprehension and verdict deliberation is a defensible one. (Author/FL)
Descriptors: Behavior Patterns, Cognitive Processes, Communication (Thought Transfer), Decision Making
Schaffer, William O.; LaBerge, David – Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1979
Two experiments were conducted to test how readers process unattended words in a display and how the semantic category of these flanking words affects response time. (Author/AM)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Language Research, Psycholinguistics, Reading Processes
Yekovich, Frank R.; And Others – Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1979
Two experiments examined the role of presupposed (or old) and focal (or new) information in integrating sentences, by measuring comprehension time for various combinations of presupposed and focal information. (Author/AM)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Language Processing, Language Research
Roediger, Henry L., III; Tulving, Endel – Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1979
Three experiments are reported in which, following presentation of a categorized list, subjects either recalled the whole list or a part of the list. Results indicate that it is difficult to retrieve selectively parts of a studied list when instructions specify only what not to recall. (Author/AM)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Experimental Psychology, Language Research, Learning Processes

Zwicky, Arnold M. – Language Sciences, 1979
Examines 158 examples of malapropisms and determines three possible sources of this type of error: (1) childhood errors that were never corrected, (2) other kinds of imperfect learning, and (3) breakdown in the storage and retrieval system of the mental lexicon. (AM)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Error Analysis (Language), Error Patterns, Language Processing
Gardiner, John M. – Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1980
Two experiments showed the proportion of recalled words recognized to be higher than expected when the experiment was conducted under typical study conditions. Under special study conditions, the proportion of recalled words recognized more closely approximated expected values. Exceptions depend on encoding operations rather than on the properties…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Language Processing, Language Research, Memory
Akiyama, M. Michael; And Others – Journal of Verbal Learning and Verbal Behavior, 1979
Two experiments investigated the relationship of verification to the answering of yes-no questions. Subjects verified simple statements or answered simple questions. Various proposals concerning the relative difficulty of answering questions and verifying statements were considered, and a model was proposed. (SW)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Discourse Analysis, Language Research, Learning Processes

Wilcox, Sherman – Sign Language Studies, 1990
A review of research and theory on the structure of signed and spoken languages explores the relationship between the two language types and how the study of signed languages can inform researchers about the human capacity for language. (29 references) (CB)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Contrastive Linguistics, Finger Spelling, Language Patterns

Crookes, Graham – Studies in Second Language Acquisition, 1989
Reports on an experiment in which 2 groups of 20 Japanese learners of English as a Second Language performed 2 monologic production tasks with and without time for planning. It was found that providing learners with time to plan their utterances results in interlanguage productions that are more complex. (64 references) (Author/VWL)
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, English (Second Language), Higher Education, Interlanguage

Swaffar, Janet K. – Modern Language Journal, 1989
Reviews changes in the approach to language learning that have taken place during the past 50 years. A new relationship between cognition and language has led to new ways of researching language learning and indicates the need for new curriculum that meet the needs of the adult second language learner. (76 references) (Author/OD)
Descriptors: Adult Students, Cognitive Processes, Curriculum Development, Language Research

Spivey-Knowlton, Michael; Sedivy, Julie C. – Cognition, 1995
Through analyses of text corpora, sentence completion, and self-paced reading, examined role of structurally defined parsing principles, local information (lexically specific biases), and contextual information (referential pragmatics) in resolving syntactic ambiguities. Subjects were 32 undergraduate native English speakers. Found that local and…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Error Analysis (Language), Language Processing, Language Research

Oxford, Rebecca L.; Cohen, Andrew D. – Applied Language Learning, 1992
This article notes seven key problems in language learning strategy concepts and classification systems as they relate to empirical research in the area of second- and foreign language development and suggests ways to resolve these problems and thereby increase unity, coherence, and meaningfulness of language learning strategy research. (99…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Communicative Competence (Languages), Language Research, Learning Strategies