Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 18 |
Imagination | 18 |
Pretend Play | 18 |
Early Childhood Education | 9 |
Cognitive Development | 8 |
Preschool Children | 5 |
Young Children | 5 |
Play | 4 |
Autism | 3 |
Child Development | 3 |
Fantasy | 3 |
More ▼ |
Source
Developmental Psychology | 3 |
Child Development | 2 |
Childhood Education | 1 |
Early Child Development and… | 1 |
International Journal of… | 1 |
Journal of Autism and… | 1 |
Psychological Review | 1 |
Psychology in the Schools | 1 |
Author
Harris, Paul L. | 2 |
Lillard, Angeline S. | 2 |
Adcock, Don | 1 |
Berk, Laura E. | 1 |
Clark, Cindy Dell | 1 |
Frith, Uta | 1 |
Galasso, Lisa | 1 |
Golomb, Claire | 1 |
Kavanaugh, Robert, D. | 1 |
Leslie, Alan M. | 1 |
Marbach, Ellen S. | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Education Level
Audience
Location
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
What Works Clearinghouse Rating

Lillard, Angeline S. – Child Development, 1993
Investigates whether pretend play is an area of advanced understanding with reference to certain skills that are implicated in both pretend play and a theory of mind, including the ability to (1) represent one object as two things at once; (2) see one object as representing another; and (3) represent mental representations. (MDM)
Descriptors: Children, Cognitive Processes, Early Childhood Education, Imagination

Marbach, Ellen S.; Yawkey, Thomas Daniels – Psychology in the Schools, 1980
Analysis (using semantic scoring criteria) indicated that: (1) self-action yielded higher scores on recall; and (2) girls scored significantly higher than boys. When absolute and syntactic criteria were used, self-action, puppet-action, and color, paste, and cut actions were equally facilitative. (Author)
Descriptors: Aural Learning, Cognitive Processes, Imagination, Language Acquisition

Walker-Andrews, Arlene S.; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Psychology, 1993
Two experiments assessed preschoolers' ability to understand pretend transformations. Subjects were two-, three-, and four-year-olds who viewed episodes in which either one or two similar props were altered in a pretend fashion. In both the single and double transformation, children demonstrated that they could keep track of the pretend…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Comprehension, Early Childhood Education

Kavanaugh, Robert, D.; Harris, Paul L. – Developmental Psychology, 1994
Studied children's grasp of make-believe transformations they had seen enacted. Children indicated the pretend outcome by choosing a picture depicting no change or a picture depicting the pretend change. Older children chose correctly, even with the addition of a picture of an irrelevant transformation, but younger children did not. Autistic…
Descriptors: Abstract Reasoning, Age Differences, Autism, Cognitive Development

Mellou, Eleni – Early Child Development and Care, 1995
Examines the differences between imagination, creativity, and fantasy, and presents the relationship of imagination to creativity. Suggests that the basic distinction between imagination and fantasy is that while imagination is related to reality, fantasy is related to unreality. The link between imagination and creativity lies in the opportunity…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Concept Formation, Creative Thinking, Creativity
Nicolich, Lorraine McCune – 1978
This article provides a comparative analysis of studies in which symbolic play in children ages 1 through 3 was the major focus of a formal research strategy. The review provides readers with (1) information allowing more effective evaluation of research involving symbolic play and (2) background for designing or adopting play measurement…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Stages, Imagination

Golomb, Claire; Galasso, Lisa – Developmental Psychology, 1995
Two studies examined 19 preschoolers' ability to distinguish between pretense and reality, testing whether emotionally charged events can cause children to lend probability to pretense. Subjects were assigned to various conditions including termination or no termination of pretense and emotionally colored pretense play scenarios. Found that, even…
Descriptors: Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Emotional Development, Emotional Response

Trawick-Smith, Jeffrey; Picard, Theresa – Childhood Education, 2003
Raises concerns about whether literacy-enriched play in early childhood settings is really play. Presents a vignette to illustrate how a teacher can model literacy unobtrusively, thereby enhancing literacy, but unwittingly draw children away from meaningful play activities. Differentiates the cognitive processes involved in play and literacy…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Early Childhood Education, Emergent Literacy, Imagination

Rutherford, M. D.; Rogers, Sally J. – Journal of Autism and Developmental Disorders, 2003
A study examined the cognitive underpinnings of spontaneous and prompted pretend play in 28 children with autism (ages 2-3), 24 children with developmental disorders, and 26 controls (ages 1-3). Children with autism were significantly delayed on pretend play scores. They also had significant deficits in a theory of mind measure. (Contains…
Descriptors: Autism, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Delays
Segal, Marilyn; Adcock, Don – 1982
By participating in their children's imaginative play or pretending, parents may be able to understand better their children's feelings, resolve parent-child conflicts, communicate parental values, and build parent-child relationships based on mutual respect. Many people seem to believe that pretending appears automatically in young children, that…
Descriptors: Cognitive Processes, Coping, Early Childhood Education, Imagination

Lillard, Angeline S. – Child Development, 1993
Four experiments confirmed the widely accepted hypothesis that, although children as young as two engage in pretend play, even four and five year olds do not understand that pretending requires mental representation. Children appear to misconstrue pretense as its common external manifestations, such as actions, until at least age six. (MDM)
Descriptors: Age Differences, Cognitive Processes, Developmental Stages, Early Childhood Education

Weininger, Otto – International Journal of Early Childhood, 1986
Through examples of both a child's imagination and pretend play activities, demonstrates how a child's imagination is the thinking function that sets the stage for play, while actual play consists of a child's understanding and representation of reality. (HOD)
Descriptors: Behavior Development, Child Development, Cognitive Development, Cognitive Processes

Leslie, Alan M.; Frith, Uta – Psychological Review, 1990
Three possibilities for the pathogenesis of childhood autism are considered. Little evidence exists for autism as a basic affective disorder, as proposed by R. P. Hobson, but growing evidence supports childhood autism as a basic cognitive disorder, as the author argues. The author's original theoretical perspectives are revised. (SLD)
Descriptors: Affective Behavior, Autism, Behavior Disorders, Child Development
Clark, Cindy Dell – 1996
This ethnographic study used multiple approaches to try to determine the emotional experience of young children (ages 5 to 8) with chronic illnesses. Forty-six children with severe asthma and diabetes were interviewed on two separate occasions using child-centered in-depth interviews that included play-based interviewing. The study also employed…
Descriptors: Asthma, Childhood Attitudes, Chronic Illness, Cognitive Processes
Robertson, J. D. – 1977
This pamphlet is the eighth in a series of ten stemming from the view that language is central to learning, that teachers can gain insights into their work and into learning by examining the language of the classroom, and that current language theory can be the means to such insights. The pamphlet contains a discussion of the uses of language in…
Descriptors: Child Language, Cognitive Processes, Expressive Language, Fiction
Previous Page | Next Page ยป
Pages: 1 | 2