Publication Date
In 2025 | 0 |
Since 2024 | 0 |
Since 2021 (last 5 years) | 1 |
Since 2016 (last 10 years) | 4 |
Since 2006 (last 20 years) | 22 |
Descriptor
Cognitive Processes | 23 |
Correlation | 23 |
Familiarity | 23 |
Recognition (Psychology) | 10 |
Brain Hemisphere Functions | 6 |
Foreign Countries | 6 |
Age Differences | 5 |
Brain | 5 |
Children | 4 |
Comparative Analysis | 4 |
Diagnostic Tests | 4 |
More ▼ |
Source
Author
Carver, Leslie J. | 2 |
Sweet, Monica A. | 2 |
Swingler, Margaret M. | 2 |
Ahmadiantehrani, Somayeh | 1 |
Autry, Kevin S. | 1 |
Balota, David A. | 1 |
Boets, Bart | 1 |
Clerc, Olivier | 1 |
Cornoldi, Cesare | 1 |
Del Prete, Francesco | 1 |
Esch, Megan | 1 |
More ▼ |
Publication Type
Journal Articles | 22 |
Reports - Research | 19 |
Reports - Evaluative | 3 |
Dissertations/Theses -… | 1 |
Tests/Questionnaires | 1 |
Education Level
Early Childhood Education | 3 |
Kindergarten | 2 |
Preschool Education | 2 |
Primary Education | 2 |
Elementary Education | 1 |
Audience
Laws, Policies, & Programs
Assessments and Surveys
Peabody Picture Vocabulary… | 1 |
SAT (College Admission Test) | 1 |
What Works Clearinghouse Rating
Autry, Kevin S.; Jordan, Tessa M.; Girgis, Helana; Falcon, Rachael G. – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2020
The abstract concept of time is conceptualized as moving linearly across space, known as the mental timeline (MTL). The direction of our MTL is consistent with reading direction. English speakers, who read left to right, think of past on the left and future on the right; the reverse is true of Hebrew speakers, who read right to left. However, it…
Descriptors: English, Native Language, Preschool Children, Kindergarten
Clerc, Olivier; Fort, Mathilde; Schwarzer, Gudrun; Krasotkina, Anna; Vilain, Anne; Méary, David; Loevenbruck, Hélène; Pascalis, Olivier – International Journal of Behavioral Development, 2022
Between 6 and 9 months, while infant's ability to discriminate faces within their own racial group is maintained, discrimination of faces within other-race groups declines to a point where 9-month-old infants fail to discriminate other-race faces. Such face perception narrowing can be overcome in various ways at 9 or 12 months of age, such as…
Descriptors: Human Body, Infants, Recognition (Psychology), Race
Ahmadiantehrani, Somayeh; Gores, Elisa O.; London, Sarah E. – Learning & Memory, 2018
Nonassociative learning is considered simple because it depends on presentation of a single stimulus, but it likely reflects complex molecular signaling. To advance understanding of the molecular mechanisms of one form of nonassociative learning, habituation, for ethologically relevant signals we examined song recognition learning in adult zebra…
Descriptors: Habituation, Associative Learning, Correlation, Singing
Michel, Christine; Kaduk, Katharina; Ní Choisdealbha, Áine; Reid, Vincent M. – Developmental Psychology, 2017
Previous event-related potential (ERP) work has indicated that the neural processing of action sequences develops with age. Although adults and 9-month-olds use a semantic processing system, perceiving actions activates attentional processes in 7-month-olds. However, presenting a sequence of action context, action execution and action conclusion…
Descriptors: Infants, Adults, Age Differences, Cognitive Processes
Greene, Ciara M.; Vidaki, Kleio; Soto, David – Learning & Memory, 2015
Familiar stimuli are typically accompanied by decreases in neural response relative to the presentation of novel items, but these studies often include explicit instructions to discriminate old and new items; this creates difficulties in partialling out the contribution of top-down intentional orientation to the items based on recognition goals.…
Descriptors: Recognition (Psychology), Incidental Learning, Familiarity, Stimuli
Guillaume, Fabrice; Tiberghien, Guy – Brain and Cognition, 2013
The present study investigated the impact of study-test similarity on face recognition by manipulating, in the same experiment, the expression change (same vs. different) and the task-processing context (inclusion vs. exclusion instructions) as within-subject variables. Consistent with the dual-process framework, the present results showed that…
Descriptors: Memory, Brain Hemisphere Functions, Recognition (Psychology), Cognitive Processes
Del Prete, Francesco; Mirandola, Chiara; Konishi, Mahiko; Cornoldi, Cesare; Ghetti, Simona – Journal of Cognition and Development, 2014
The effects of warning on false recognition and associated subjective experience of false recollection and familiarity were investigated in 7-to 13-year-old children and young adults (N = 259) using the Deese-Roediger-McDermott (DRM) paradigm. Two warning conditions (warning with an example of a critical lure and warning without an example of a…
Descriptors: Children, Adolescents, Familiarity, Control Groups
Lucas, Heather D.; Taylor, Jason R.; Henson, Richard N.; Paller, Ken A. – Neuropsychologia, 2012
The neural mechanisms that underlie familiarity memory have been extensively investigated, but a consensus understanding remains elusive. Behavioral evidence suggests that familiarity sometimes shares sources with instances of implicit memory known as priming, in that the same increases in processing fluency that give rise to priming can engender…
Descriptors: Evidence, Familiarity, Recognition (Psychology), Priming
Olds, Justin M.; Westerman, Deanne L. – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2012
Stimuli that are processed fluently tend to be regarded as more familiar and are more likely to be classified as old on a recognition test compared with less fluent stimuli. Recently it was shown that the standard relationship between fluency and positive recognition judgments can be reversed if participants are trained that previously studied…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Recall (Psychology), Recognition (Psychology), Feedback (Response)
Veispak, Anneli; Boets, Bart; Mannamaa, Mairi; Ghesquiere, Pol – Research in Developmental Disabilities: A Multidisciplinary Journal, 2012
Similar to many sighted children who struggle with learning to read, a proportion of blind children have specific difficulties related to reading braille which cannot be easily explained. A lot of research has been conducted to investigate the perceptual and cognitive processes behind (impairments in) print reading. Very few studies, however, have…
Descriptors: Blindness, Braille, Reading Achievement, Familiarity
Kaushanskaya, Margarita; Yoo, Jeewon – Language and Cognitive Processes, 2011
The goal of this research was to examine the effects of phonological familiarity and rehearsal method (vocal vs. subvocal) on novel word learning. In Experiment 1, English-speaking adults learned phonologically familiar novel words that followed English phonological structure. Participants learned half the words via vocal rehearsal (saying the…
Descriptors: Speech Communication, Familiarity, Cognitive Processes, Novelty (Stimulus Dimension)
Ford, Jaclyn Hennessey; Verfaellie, Mieke; Giovanello, Kelly S. – Neuropsychologia, 2010
The current study compared the neural correlates of associative retrieval of compound (unitized) stimuli and unrelated (non-unitized) stimuli. Although associative recognition was nearly identical for compounds and unrelated pairs, accurate recognition of these different pair types was associated with activation in distinct regions within the…
Descriptors: Correlation, Familiarity, Recognition (Psychology), Comparative Analysis
Oztekin, Ilke; McElree, Brian – Journal of Experimental Psychology: Learning, Memory, and Cognition, 2010
The response-signal speed-accuracy trade-off (SAT) procedure was used to investigate the relationship between measures of working memory capacity and the time course of short-term item recognition. High- and low-span participants studied sequentially presented 6-item lists, immediately followed by a recognition probe. Analyses of composite list…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Serial Ordering, Short Term Memory, Correlation
McCabe, David P.; Roediger, Henry L., III; McDaniel, Mark A.; Balota, David A. – Neuropsychologia, 2009
In 1985 Tulving introduced the remember-know procedure, whereby subjects are asked to distinguish between memories that involve retrieval of contextual details (remembering) and memories that do not (knowing). Several studies have been reported showing age-related declines in remember hits, which has typically been interpreted as supporting…
Descriptors: Familiarity, Memory, Age Differences, Aging (Individuals)
Stenberg, Georg; Hellman, Johan; Johansson, Mikael; Rosen, Ingmar – Journal of Cognitive Neuroscience, 2009
Recent interest has been drawn to the separate components of recognition memory, as studied by event-related potentials (ERPs). In ERPs, recollection is usually accompanied by a late, parietal positive deflection. An earlier, frontal component has been suggested to be a counterpart, accompanying recognition by familiarity. However, this component,…
Descriptors: Reputation, Familiarity, Recognition (Psychology), Priming
Previous Page | Next Page »
Pages: 1 | 2